Steven Pinker: What our language habits reveal

441,570 views ・ 2007-09-11

TED


Please double-click on the English subtitles below to play the video.

00:26
This is a picture of Maurice Druon,
0
26000
2000
00:28
the Honorary Perpetual Secretary of L'Academie francaise,
1
28000
4000
00:32
the French Academy.
2
32000
2000
00:34
He is splendidly attired in his 68,000-dollar uniform,
3
34000
5000
00:39
befitting the role of the French Academy
4
39000
3000
00:42
as legislating the
5
42000
3000
00:45
correct usage in French
6
45000
2000
00:47
and perpetuating the language.
7
47000
2000
00:49
The French Academy has two main tasks:
8
49000
3000
00:52
it compiles a dictionary of official French.
9
52000
3000
00:55
They're now working on their ninth edition,
10
55000
3000
00:58
which they began in 1930, and they've reached the letter P.
11
58000
3000
01:02
They also legislate on correct usage,
12
62000
3000
01:05
such as the proper term for what the French call "email,"
13
65000
4000
01:09
which ought to be "courriel."
14
69000
2000
01:11
The World Wide Web, the French are told,
15
71000
2000
01:13
ought to be referred to as
16
73000
2000
01:15
"la toile d'araignee mondiale" -- the Global Spider Web --
17
75000
4000
01:19
recommendations that the French gaily ignore.
18
79000
4000
01:24
Now, this is one model of how language comes to be:
19
84000
4000
01:28
namely, it's legislated by an academy.
20
88000
3000
01:31
But anyone who looks at language realizes
21
91000
3000
01:34
that this is a rather silly conceit,
22
94000
4000
01:38
that language, rather, emerges from human minds interacting from one another.
23
98000
3000
01:41
And this is visible in the unstoppable change in language --
24
101000
4000
01:45
the fact that by the time the Academy finishes their dictionary,
25
105000
3000
01:48
it will already be well out of date.
26
108000
2000
01:50
We see it in the
27
110000
2000
01:52
constant appearance of slang and jargon,
28
112000
4000
01:56
of the historical change in languages,
29
116000
2000
01:58
in divergence of dialects
30
118000
2000
02:00
and the formation of new languages.
31
120000
3000
02:03
So language is not so much a creator or shaper of human nature,
32
123000
3000
02:06
so much as a window onto human nature.
33
126000
3000
02:09
In a book that I'm currently working on,
34
129000
3000
02:12
I hope to use language to shed light on
35
132000
3000
02:15
a number of aspects of human nature,
36
135000
2000
02:17
including the cognitive machinery
37
137000
2000
02:19
with which humans conceptualize the world
38
139000
3000
02:22
and the relationship types that govern human interaction.
39
142000
3000
02:25
And I'm going to say a few words about each one this morning.
40
145000
3000
02:28
Let me start off with a technical problem in language
41
148000
2000
02:30
that I've worried about for quite some time --
42
150000
2000
02:32
and indulge me
43
152000
4000
02:36
in my passion for verbs and how they're used.
44
156000
3000
02:39
The problem is, which verbs go in which constructions?
45
159000
3000
02:42
The verb is the chassis of the sentence.
46
162000
3000
02:45
It's the framework onto which the other parts are bolted.
47
165000
4000
02:49
Let me give you a quick reminder
48
169000
2000
02:51
of something that you've long forgotten.
49
171000
2000
02:53
An intransitive verb, such as "dine," for example,
50
173000
3000
02:56
can't take a direct object.
51
176000
2000
02:58
You have to say, "Sam dined," not, "Sam dined the pizza."
52
178000
3000
03:01
A transitive verb mandates
53
181000
2000
03:03
that there has to be an object there:
54
183000
2000
03:05
"Sam devoured the pizza." You can't just say, "Sam devoured."
55
185000
3000
03:08
There are dozens or scores of verbs of this type,
56
188000
4000
03:12
each of which shapes its sentence.
57
192000
2000
03:14
So, a problem in explaining how children learn language,
58
194000
4000
03:18
a problem in teaching language to adults so that they don't make grammatical errors,
59
198000
5000
03:23
and a problem in programming computers to use language is
60
203000
3000
03:26
which verbs go in which constructions.
61
206000
2000
03:29
For example, the dative construction in English.
62
209000
2000
03:31
You can say, "Give a muffin to a mouse," the prepositional dative.
63
211000
3000
03:34
Or, "Give a mouse a muffin," the double-object dative.
64
214000
3000
03:37
"Promise anything to her," "Promise her anything," and so on.
65
217000
4000
03:41
Hundreds of verbs can go both ways.
66
221000
2000
03:43
So a tempting generalization for a child,
67
223000
2000
03:45
for an adult, for a computer
68
225000
2000
03:47
is that any verb that can appear in the construction,
69
227000
2000
03:49
"subject-verb-thing-to-a-recipient"
70
229000
3000
03:52
can also be expressed as "subject-verb-recipient-thing."
71
232000
3000
03:55
A handy thing to have,
72
235000
2000
03:57
because language is infinite,
73
237000
2000
03:59
and you can't just parrot back the sentences that you've heard.
74
239000
3000
04:02
You've got to extract generalizations
75
242000
2000
04:04
so you can produce and understand new sentences.
76
244000
3000
04:07
This would be an example of how to do that.
77
247000
2000
04:09
Unfortunately, there appear to be idiosyncratic exceptions.
78
249000
3000
04:12
You can say, "Biff drove the car to Chicago,"
79
252000
3000
04:15
but not, "Biff drove Chicago the car."
80
255000
3000
04:18
You can say, "Sal gave Jason a headache,"
81
258000
3000
04:21
but it's a bit odd to say, "Sal gave a headache to Jason."
82
261000
2000
04:24
The solution is that these constructions, despite initial appearance,
83
264000
3000
04:27
are not synonymous,
84
267000
2000
04:29
that when you crank up the microscope
85
269000
2000
04:31
on human cognition, you see that there's a subtle difference
86
271000
2000
04:33
in meaning between them.
87
273000
2000
04:35
So, "give the X to the Y,"
88
275000
2000
04:37
that construction corresponds to the thought
89
277000
3000
04:40
"cause X to go to Y." Whereas "give the Y the X"
90
280000
3000
04:43
corresponds to the thought "cause Y to have X."
91
283000
4000
04:47
Now, many events can be subject to either construal,
92
287000
4000
04:51
kind of like the classic figure-ground reversal illusions,
93
291000
3000
04:54
in which you can either pay attention
94
294000
3000
04:57
to the particular object,
95
297000
2000
04:59
in which case the space around it recedes from attention,
96
299000
4000
05:03
or you can see the faces in the empty space,
97
303000
2000
05:05
in which case the object recedes out of consciousness.
98
305000
4000
05:09
How are these construals reflected in language?
99
309000
2000
05:11
Well, in both cases, the thing that is construed as being affected
100
311000
4000
05:15
is expressed as the direct object,
101
315000
2000
05:17
the noun after the verb.
102
317000
2000
05:19
So, when you think of the event as causing the muffin to go somewhere --
103
319000
4000
05:23
where you're doing something to the muffin --
104
323000
2000
05:25
you say, "Give the muffin to the mouse."
105
325000
2000
05:27
When you construe it as "cause the mouse to have something,"
106
327000
3000
05:30
you're doing something to the mouse,
107
330000
2000
05:32
and therefore you express it as, "Give the mouse the muffin."
108
332000
3000
05:35
So which verbs go in which construction --
109
335000
2000
05:37
the problem with which I began --
110
337000
2000
05:39
depends on whether the verb specifies a kind of motion
111
339000
4000
05:43
or a kind of possession change.
112
343000
2000
05:45
To give something involves both causing something to go
113
345000
3000
05:48
and causing someone to have.
114
348000
2000
05:50
To drive the car only causes something to go,
115
350000
3000
05:53
because Chicago's not the kind of thing that can possess something.
116
353000
2000
05:55
Only humans can possess things.
117
355000
3000
05:58
And to give someone a headache causes them to have the headache,
118
358000
2000
06:00
but it's not as if you're taking the headache out of your head
119
360000
3000
06:03
and causing it to go to the other person,
120
363000
2000
06:05
and implanting it in them.
121
365000
2000
06:07
You may just be loud or obnoxious,
122
367000
2000
06:09
or some other way causing them to have the headache.
123
369000
2000
06:11
So, that's
124
371000
4000
06:15
an example of the kind of thing that I do in my day job.
125
375000
2000
06:17
So why should anyone care?
126
377000
2000
06:19
Well, there are a number of interesting conclusions, I think,
127
379000
3000
06:22
from this and many similar kinds of analyses
128
382000
4000
06:26
of hundreds of English verbs.
129
386000
2000
06:28
First, there's a level of fine-grained conceptual structure,
130
388000
3000
06:31
which we automatically and unconsciously compute
131
391000
3000
06:34
every time we produce or utter a sentence, that governs our use of language.
132
394000
4000
06:38
You can think of this as the language of thought, or "mentalese."
133
398000
4000
06:42
It seems to be based on a fixed set of concepts,
134
402000
3000
06:45
which govern dozens of constructions and thousands of verbs --
135
405000
3000
06:48
not only in English, but in all other languages --
136
408000
3000
06:51
fundamental concepts such as space,
137
411000
2000
06:53
time, causation and human intention,
138
413000
3000
06:56
such as, what is the means and what is the ends?
139
416000
3000
06:59
These are reminiscent of the kinds of categories
140
419000
2000
07:01
that Immanuel Kant argued
141
421000
2000
07:03
are the basic framework for human thought,
142
423000
3000
07:06
and it's interesting that our unconscious use of language
143
426000
3000
07:09
seems to reflect these Kantian categories.
144
429000
3000
07:12
Doesn't care about perceptual qualities,
145
432000
2000
07:14
such as color, texture, weight and speed,
146
434000
2000
07:16
which virtually never differentiate
147
436000
2000
07:18
the use of verbs in different constructions.
148
438000
2000
07:21
An additional twist is that all of the constructions in English
149
441000
3000
07:24
are used not only literally,
150
444000
2000
07:26
but in a quasi-metaphorical way.
151
446000
3000
07:29
For example, this construction, the dative,
152
449000
2000
07:31
is used not only to transfer things,
153
451000
2000
07:33
but also for the metaphorical transfer of ideas,
154
453000
3000
07:36
as when we say, "She told a story to me"
155
456000
2000
07:38
or "told me a story,"
156
458000
2000
07:40
"Max taught Spanish to the students" or "taught the students Spanish."
157
460000
3000
07:43
It's exactly the same construction,
158
463000
2000
07:45
but no muffins, no mice, nothing moving at all.
159
465000
4000
07:49
It evokes the container metaphor of communication,
160
469000
3000
07:52
in which we conceive of ideas as objects,
161
472000
2000
07:54
sentences as containers,
162
474000
2000
07:56
and communication as a kind of sending.
163
476000
2000
07:58
As when we say we "gather" our ideas, to "put" them "into" words,
164
478000
3000
08:01
and if our words aren't "empty" or "hollow,"
165
481000
2000
08:03
we might get these ideas "across" to a listener,
166
483000
3000
08:06
who can "unpack" our words to "extract" their "content."
167
486000
3000
08:09
And indeed, this kind of verbiage is not the exception, but the rule.
168
489000
3000
08:12
It's very hard to find any example of abstract language
169
492000
3000
08:15
that is not based on some concrete metaphor.
170
495000
3000
08:18
For example, you can use the verb "go"
171
498000
3000
08:21
and the prepositions "to" and "from"
172
501000
2000
08:23
in a literal, spatial sense.
173
503000
2000
08:25
"The messenger went from Paris to Istanbul."
174
505000
2000
08:27
You can also say, "Biff went from sick to well."
175
507000
3000
08:30
He needn't go anywhere. He could have been in bed the whole time,
176
510000
3000
08:33
but it's as if his health is a point in state space
177
513000
2000
08:35
that you conceptualize as moving.
178
515000
2000
08:37
Or, "The meeting went from three to four,"
179
517000
2000
08:39
in which we conceive of time as stretched along a line.
180
519000
3000
08:42
Likewise, we use "force" to indicate
181
522000
3000
08:45
not only physical force,
182
525000
2000
08:47
as in, "Rose forced the door to open,"
183
527000
2000
08:49
but also interpersonal force,
184
529000
2000
08:51
as in, "Rose forced Sadie to go," not necessarily by manhandling her,
185
531000
4000
08:55
but by issuing a threat.
186
535000
2000
08:57
Or, "Rose forced herself to go,"
187
537000
2000
08:59
as if there were two entities inside Rose's head,
188
539000
2000
09:02
engaged in a tug of a war.
189
542000
2000
09:04
Second conclusion is that the ability to conceive
190
544000
3000
09:07
of a given event in two different ways,
191
547000
3000
09:10
such as "cause something to go to someone"
192
550000
2000
09:12
and "causing someone to have something,"
193
552000
2000
09:14
I think is a fundamental feature of human thought,
194
554000
4000
09:18
and it's the basis for much human argumentation,
195
558000
3000
09:21
in which people don't differ so much on the facts
196
561000
3000
09:24
as on how they ought to be construed.
197
564000
2000
09:26
Just to give you a few examples:
198
566000
2000
09:28
"ending a pregnancy" versus "killing a fetus;"
199
568000
2000
09:30
"a ball of cells" versus "an unborn child;"
200
570000
3000
09:33
"invading Iraq" versus "liberating Iraq;"
201
573000
2000
09:35
"redistributing wealth" versus "confiscating earnings."
202
575000
4000
09:39
And I think the biggest picture of all
203
579000
2000
09:41
would take seriously the fact
204
581000
3000
09:44
that so much of our verbiage about abstract events
205
584000
3000
09:47
is based on a concrete metaphor
206
587000
2000
09:49
and see human intelligence itself
207
589000
2000
09:51
as consisting of a repertoire of concepts --
208
591000
3000
09:54
such as objects, space, time, causation and intention --
209
594000
3000
09:57
which are useful in a social, knowledge-intensive species,
210
597000
4000
10:01
whose evolution you can well imagine,
211
601000
2000
10:03
and a process of metaphorical abstraction
212
603000
3000
10:06
that allows us to bleach these concepts
213
606000
2000
10:08
of their original conceptual content --
214
608000
3000
10:11
space, time and force --
215
611000
3000
10:14
and apply them to new abstract domains,
216
614000
2000
10:16
therefore allowing a species that evolved
217
616000
3000
10:19
to deal with rocks and tools and animals,
218
619000
2000
10:21
to conceptualize mathematics, physics, law
219
621000
3000
10:24
and other abstract domains.
220
624000
3000
10:27
Well, I said I'd talk about two windows on human nature --
221
627000
3000
10:30
the cognitive machinery with which we conceptualize the world,
222
630000
3000
10:33
and now I'm going to say a few words about the relationship types
223
633000
2000
10:35
that govern human social interaction,
224
635000
2000
10:37
again, as reflected in language.
225
637000
2000
10:40
And I'll start out with a puzzle, the puzzle of indirect speech acts.
226
640000
4000
10:44
Now, I'm sure most of you have seen the movie "Fargo."
227
644000
2000
10:46
And you might remember the scene in which
228
646000
2000
10:48
the kidnapper is pulled over by a police officer,
229
648000
3000
10:51
is asked to show his driver's license
230
651000
2000
10:53
and holds his wallet out
231
653000
2000
10:55
with a 50-dollar bill extending
232
655000
3000
10:58
at a slight angle out of the wallet.
233
658000
2000
11:00
And he says, "I was just thinking
234
660000
2000
11:02
that maybe we could take care of it here in Fargo,"
235
662000
2000
11:04
which everyone, including the audience,
236
664000
3000
11:07
interprets as a veiled bribe.
237
667000
3000
11:10
This kind of indirect speech is rampant in language.
238
670000
4000
11:14
For example, in polite requests,
239
674000
2000
11:16
if someone says, "If you could pass the guacamole,
240
676000
2000
11:18
that would be awesome,"
241
678000
2000
11:20
we know exactly what he means,
242
680000
2000
11:22
even though that's a rather bizarre
243
682000
2000
11:24
concept being expressed.
244
684000
2000
11:26
(Laughter)
245
686000
3000
11:29
"Would you like to come up and see my etchings?"
246
689000
2000
11:31
I think most people
247
691000
2000
11:33
understand the intent behind that.
248
693000
3000
11:36
And likewise, if someone says,
249
696000
2000
11:38
"Nice store you've got there. It would be a real shame if something happened to it" --
250
698000
3000
11:41
(Laughter) --
251
701000
1000
11:42
we understand that as a veiled threat,
252
702000
2000
11:44
rather than a musing of hypothetical possibilities.
253
704000
3000
11:47
So the puzzle is, why are bribes,
254
707000
3000
11:50
polite requests, solicitations and threats so often veiled?
255
710000
3000
11:53
No one's fooled.
256
713000
2000
11:55
Both parties know exactly what the speaker means,
257
715000
3000
11:58
and the speaker knows the listener knows
258
718000
2000
12:00
that the speaker knows that the listener knows, etc., etc.
259
720000
3000
12:03
So what's going on?
260
723000
2000
12:05
I think the key idea is that language
261
725000
2000
12:07
is a way of negotiating relationships,
262
727000
2000
12:09
and human relationships fall into a number of types.
263
729000
3000
12:12
There's an influential taxonomy by the anthropologist Alan Fiske,
264
732000
4000
12:16
in which relationships can be categorized, more or less,
265
736000
3000
12:19
into communality, which works on the principle
266
739000
2000
12:21
"what's mine is thine, what's thine is mine,"
267
741000
3000
12:24
the kind of mindset that operates within a family, for example;
268
744000
4000
12:28
dominance, whose principle is "don't mess with me;"
269
748000
3000
12:31
reciprocity, "you scratch my back, I'll scratch yours;"
270
751000
4000
12:35
and sexuality, in the immortal words of Cole Porter, "Let's do it."
271
755000
5000
12:40
Now, relationship types can be negotiated.
272
760000
3000
12:43
Even though there are default situations
273
763000
3000
12:46
in which one of these mindsets can be applied,
274
766000
2000
12:48
they can be stretched and extended.
275
768000
3000
12:51
For example, communality applies most naturally
276
771000
3000
12:54
within family or friends,
277
774000
2000
12:56
but it can be used to try to transfer
278
776000
2000
12:58
the mentality of sharing
279
778000
2000
13:00
to groups that ordinarily would not be disposed to exercise it.
280
780000
4000
13:04
For example, in brotherhoods, fraternal organizations,
281
784000
4000
13:08
sororities, locutions like "the family of man,"
282
788000
3000
13:11
you try to get people who are not related
283
791000
2000
13:13
to use the relationship type that would ordinarily
284
793000
4000
13:17
be appropriate to close kin.
285
797000
2000
13:19
Now, mismatches -- when one person assumes one relationship type,
286
799000
3000
13:22
and another assumes a different one -- can be awkward.
287
802000
3000
13:25
If you went over and you helped yourself
288
805000
2000
13:27
to a shrimp off your boss' plate,
289
807000
2000
13:29
for example, that would be an awkward situation.
290
809000
2000
13:31
Or if a dinner guest after the meal
291
811000
2000
13:33
pulled out his wallet and offered to pay you for the meal,
292
813000
3000
13:36
that would be rather awkward as well.
293
816000
2000
13:38
In less blatant cases,
294
818000
3000
13:41
there's still a kind of negotiation that often goes on.
295
821000
3000
13:44
In the workplace, for example,
296
824000
2000
13:46
there's often a tension over whether an employee
297
826000
2000
13:48
can socialize with the boss,
298
828000
2000
13:50
or refer to him or her
299
830000
2000
13:52
on a first-name basis.
300
832000
2000
13:54
If two friends have a
301
834000
2000
13:56
reciprocal transaction, like selling a car,
302
836000
2000
13:58
it's well known that this can be a source
303
838000
2000
14:00
of tension or awkwardness.
304
840000
2000
14:02
In dating, the transition
305
842000
2000
14:04
from friendship to sex
306
844000
2000
14:06
can lead to, notoriously, various forms of awkwardness,
307
846000
3000
14:09
and as can sex in the workplace,
308
849000
2000
14:11
in which we call the conflict between a
309
851000
2000
14:13
dominant and a sexual relationship "sexual harassment."
310
853000
4000
14:17
Well, what does this have to do with language?
311
857000
2000
14:19
Well, language, as a social interaction,
312
859000
2000
14:21
has to satisfy two conditions.
313
861000
2000
14:23
You have to convey the actual content --
314
863000
3000
14:26
here we get back to the container metaphor.
315
866000
2000
14:28
You want to express the bribe, the command, the promise,
316
868000
3000
14:31
the solicitation and so on,
317
871000
2000
14:33
but you also have to negotiate
318
873000
2000
14:35
and maintain the kind of relationship
319
875000
2000
14:37
you have with the other person.
320
877000
2000
14:39
The solution, I think, is that we use language at two levels:
321
879000
3000
14:42
the literal form signals
322
882000
2000
14:44
the safest relationship with the listener,
323
884000
2000
14:46
whereas the implicated content --
324
886000
2000
14:49
the reading between the lines that we count on the listener to perform --
325
889000
2000
14:52
allows the listener to derive the interpretation
326
892000
2000
14:54
which is most relevant in context,
327
894000
2000
14:56
which possibly initiates a changed relationship.
328
896000
3000
14:59
The simplest example of this is in the polite request.
329
899000
4000
15:03
If you express your request as a conditional --
330
903000
3000
15:06
"if you could open the window, that would be great" --
331
906000
3000
15:09
even though the content is an imperative,
332
909000
2000
15:11
the fact that you're not using the imperative voice
333
911000
2000
15:14
means that you're not acting as if you're in a relationship of dominance,
334
914000
3000
15:18
where you could presuppose the compliance of the other person.
335
918000
3000
15:21
On the other hand, you want the damn guacamole.
336
921000
2000
15:23
By expressing it as an if-then statement,
337
923000
3000
15:26
you can get the message across
338
926000
2000
15:28
without appearing to boss another person around.
339
928000
4000
15:32
And in a more subtle way, I think, this works
340
932000
2000
15:34
for all of the veiled speech acts
341
934000
2000
15:36
involving plausible deniability:
342
936000
2000
15:38
the bribes, threats, propositions,
343
938000
2000
15:40
solicitations and so on.
344
940000
2000
15:42
One way of thinking about it is to imagine what it would be like
345
942000
2000
15:44
if language -- where it could only be used literally.
346
944000
3000
15:47
And you can think of it in terms of a
347
947000
2000
15:49
game-theoretic payoff matrix.
348
949000
3000
15:52
Put yourself in the position of the
349
952000
2000
15:54
kidnapper wanting to bribe the officer.
350
954000
3000
15:57
There's a high stakes
351
957000
2000
15:59
in the two possibilities
352
959000
3000
16:02
of having a dishonest officer or an honest officer.
353
962000
3000
16:05
If you don't bribe the officer,
354
965000
3000
16:08
then you will get a traffic ticket --
355
968000
2000
16:10
or, as is the case of "Fargo," worse --
356
970000
2000
16:12
whether the honest officer
357
972000
2000
16:14
is honest or dishonest.
358
974000
2000
16:16
Nothing ventured, nothing gained.
359
976000
2000
16:18
In that case, the consequences are rather severe.
360
978000
3000
16:21
On the other hand, if you extend the bribe,
361
981000
2000
16:23
if the officer is dishonest,
362
983000
2000
16:25
you get a huge payoff of going free.
363
985000
3000
16:28
If the officer is honest, you get a huge penalty
364
988000
3000
16:31
of being arrested for bribery.
365
991000
2000
16:33
So this is a rather fraught situation.
366
993000
2000
16:35
On the other hand, with indirect language,
367
995000
2000
16:37
if you issue a veiled bribe,
368
997000
2000
16:39
then the dishonest officer
369
999000
2000
16:41
could interpret it as a bribe,
370
1001000
2000
16:43
in which case you get the payoff of going free.
371
1003000
3000
16:46
The honest officer can't hold you to it as being a bribe,
372
1006000
3000
16:49
and therefore, you get the nuisance of the traffic ticket.
373
1009000
3000
16:52
So you get the best of both worlds.
374
1012000
3000
16:55
And a similar analysis, I think,
375
1015000
2000
16:57
can apply to the potential awkwardness
376
1017000
2000
16:59
of a sexual solicitation,
377
1019000
2000
17:01
and other cases where plausible deniability is an asset.
378
1021000
3000
17:04
I think this affirms
379
1024000
2000
17:06
something that's long been known by diplomats --
380
1026000
2000
17:08
namely, that the vagueness of language,
381
1028000
2000
17:10
far from being a bug or an imperfection,
382
1030000
3000
17:13
actually might be a feature of language,
383
1033000
3000
17:16
one that we use to our advantage in social interactions.
384
1036000
3000
17:19
So to sum up: language is a collective human creation,
385
1039000
3000
17:22
reflecting human nature,
386
1042000
2000
17:24
how we conceptualize reality,
387
1044000
2000
17:26
how we relate to one another.
388
1046000
2000
17:28
And then by analyzing the various quirks and complexities of language,
389
1048000
4000
17:32
I think we can get a window onto what makes us tick.
390
1052000
3000
17:35
Thank you very much.
391
1055000
1000
17:36
(Applause)
392
1056000
1000
About this website

This site will introduce you to YouTube videos that are useful for learning English. You will see English lessons taught by top-notch teachers from around the world. Double-click on the English subtitles displayed on each video page to play the video from there. The subtitles scroll in sync with the video playback. If you have any comments or requests, please contact us using this contact form.

https://forms.gle/WvT1wiN1qDtmnspy7