How we read each other's minds | Rebecca Saxe

561,739 views ・ 2009-09-11

TED


Please double-click on the English subtitles below to play the video.

00:12
Today I'm going to talk to you about the problem of other minds.
0
12160
3000
00:15
And the problem I'm going to talk about
1
15160
2000
00:17
is not the familiar one from philosophy,
2
17160
3000
00:20
which is, "How can we know
3
20160
2000
00:22
whether other people have minds?"
4
22160
2000
00:24
That is, maybe you have a mind,
5
24160
2000
00:26
and everyone else is just a really convincing robot.
6
26160
3000
00:29
So that's a problem in philosophy,
7
29160
2000
00:31
but for today's purposes I'm going to assume
8
31160
2000
00:33
that many people in this audience have a mind,
9
33160
2000
00:35
and that I don't have to worry about this.
10
35160
2000
00:37
There is a second problem that is maybe even more familiar to us
11
37160
3000
00:40
as parents and teachers and spouses
12
40160
3000
00:43
and novelists,
13
43160
2000
00:45
which is, "Why is it so hard
14
45160
2000
00:47
to know what somebody else wants or believes?"
15
47160
2000
00:49
Or perhaps, more relevantly,
16
49160
2000
00:51
"Why is it so hard to change what somebody else wants or believes?"
17
51160
3000
00:54
I think novelists put this best.
18
54160
2000
00:56
Like Philip Roth, who said,
19
56160
2000
00:58
"And yet, what are we to do about this terribly significant business
20
58160
3000
01:01
of other people?
21
61160
2000
01:03
So ill equipped are we all,
22
63160
2000
01:05
to envision one another's interior workings
23
65160
2000
01:07
and invisible aims."
24
67160
2000
01:09
So as a teacher and as a spouse,
25
69160
3000
01:12
this is, of course, a problem I confront every day.
26
72160
2000
01:14
But as a scientist, I'm interested in a different problem of other minds,
27
74160
3000
01:17
and that is the one I'm going to introduce to you today.
28
77160
3000
01:20
And that problem is, "How is it so easy
29
80160
2000
01:22
to know other minds?"
30
82160
2000
01:24
So to start with an illustration,
31
84160
2000
01:26
you need almost no information,
32
86160
2000
01:28
one snapshot of a stranger,
33
88160
2000
01:30
to guess what this woman is thinking,
34
90160
2000
01:32
or what this man is.
35
92160
3000
01:35
And put another way, the crux of the problem is
36
95160
2000
01:37
the machine that we use for thinking about other minds,
37
97160
3000
01:40
our brain, is made up of pieces, brain cells,
38
100160
3000
01:43
that we share with all other animals, with monkeys
39
103160
2000
01:45
and mice and even sea slugs.
40
105160
3000
01:48
And yet, you put them together in a particular network,
41
108160
3000
01:51
and what you get is the capacity to write Romeo and Juliet.
42
111160
3000
01:54
Or to say, as Alan Greenspan did,
43
114160
2000
01:56
"I know you think you understand what you thought I said,
44
116160
3000
01:59
but I'm not sure you realize that what you heard
45
119160
2000
02:01
is not what I meant."
46
121160
2000
02:03
(Laughter)
47
123160
3000
02:06
So, the job of my field of cognitive neuroscience
48
126160
2000
02:08
is to stand with these ideas,
49
128160
2000
02:10
one in each hand.
50
130160
2000
02:12
And to try to understand how you can put together
51
132160
3000
02:15
simple units, simple messages over space and time, in a network,
52
135160
4000
02:19
and get this amazing human capacity to think about minds.
53
139160
4000
02:23
So I'm going to tell you three things about this today.
54
143160
3000
02:26
Obviously the whole project here is huge.
55
146160
3000
02:29
And I'm going to tell you just our first few steps
56
149160
3000
02:32
about the discovery of a special brain region
57
152160
2000
02:34
for thinking about other people's thoughts.
58
154160
2000
02:36
Some observations on the slow development of this system
59
156160
2000
02:38
as we learn how to do this difficult job.
60
158160
4000
02:42
And then finally, to show that some of the differences
61
162160
2000
02:44
between people, in how we judge others,
62
164160
3000
02:47
can be explained by differences in this brain system.
63
167160
4000
02:51
So first, the first thing I want to tell you is that
64
171160
2000
02:53
there is a brain region in the human brain, in your brains,
65
173160
3000
02:56
whose job it is to think about other people's thoughts.
66
176160
3000
02:59
This is a picture of it.
67
179160
2000
03:01
It's called the Right Temporo-Parietal Junction.
68
181160
2000
03:03
It's above and behind your right ear.
69
183160
2000
03:05
And this is the brain region you used when you saw the pictures I showed you,
70
185160
2000
03:07
or when you read Romeo and Juliet
71
187160
2000
03:09
or when you tried to understand Alan Greenspan.
72
189160
3000
03:12
And you don't use it for solving any other kinds of logical problems.
73
192160
4000
03:16
So this brain region is called the Right TPJ.
74
196160
3000
03:19
And this picture shows the average activation
75
199160
2000
03:21
in a group of what we call typical human adults.
76
201160
2000
03:23
They're MIT undergraduates.
77
203160
2000
03:25
(Laughter)
78
205160
4000
03:29
The second thing I want to say about this brain system
79
209160
2000
03:31
is that although we human adults
80
211160
2000
03:33
are really good at understanding other minds,
81
213160
2000
03:35
we weren't always that way.
82
215160
2000
03:37
It takes children a long time to break into the system.
83
217160
3000
03:40
I'm going to show you a little bit of that long, extended process.
84
220160
4000
03:44
The first thing I'm going to show you is a change between age three and five,
85
224160
3000
03:47
as kids learn to understand
86
227160
2000
03:49
that somebody else can have beliefs that are different from their own.
87
229160
3000
03:52
So I'm going to show you a five-year-old
88
232160
2000
03:54
who is getting a standard kind of puzzle
89
234160
2000
03:56
that we call the false belief task.
90
236160
3000
03:59
Rebecca Saxe (Video): This is the first pirate. His name is Ivan.
91
239160
3000
04:02
And you know what pirates really like?
92
242160
2000
04:04
Child: What? RS: Pirates really like cheese sandwiches.
93
244160
3000
04:07
Child: Cheese? I love cheese!
94
247160
3000
04:10
RS: Yeah. So Ivan has this cheese sandwich,
95
250160
2000
04:12
and he says, "Yum yum yum yum yum!
96
252160
2000
04:14
I really love cheese sandwiches."
97
254160
2000
04:16
And Ivan puts his sandwich over here, on top of the pirate chest.
98
256160
4000
04:20
And Ivan says, "You know what? I need a drink with my lunch."
99
260160
4000
04:24
And so Ivan goes to get a drink.
100
264160
3000
04:27
And while Ivan is away
101
267160
2000
04:29
the wind comes,
102
269160
3000
04:32
and it blows the sandwich down onto the grass.
103
272160
2000
04:34
And now, here comes the other pirate.
104
274160
4000
04:38
This pirate is called Joshua.
105
278160
3000
04:41
And Joshua also really loves cheese sandwiches.
106
281160
2000
04:43
So Joshua has a cheese sandwich and he says,
107
283160
2000
04:45
"Yum yum yum yum yum! I love cheese sandwiches."
108
285160
4000
04:49
And he puts his cheese sandwich over here on top of the pirate chest.
109
289160
3000
04:52
Child: So, that one is his.
110
292160
2000
04:54
RS: That one is Joshua's. That's right.
111
294160
2000
04:56
Child: And then his went on the ground.
112
296160
2000
04:58
RS: That's exactly right.
113
298160
2000
05:00
Child: So he won't know which one is his.
114
300160
2000
05:02
RS: Oh. So now Joshua goes off to get a drink.
115
302160
3000
05:05
Ivan comes back and he says, "I want my cheese sandwich."
116
305160
4000
05:09
So which one do you think Ivan is going to take?
117
309160
3000
05:12
Child: I think he is going to take that one.
118
312160
2000
05:14
RS: Yeah, you think he's going to take that one? All right. Let's see.
119
314160
2000
05:16
Oh yeah, you were right. He took that one.
120
316160
3000
05:19
So that's a five-year-old who clearly understands
121
319160
2000
05:21
that other people can have false beliefs
122
321160
2000
05:23
and what the consequences are for their actions.
123
323160
2000
05:25
Now I'm going to show you a three-year-old
124
325160
3000
05:28
who got the same puzzle.
125
328160
2000
05:30
RS: And Ivan says, "I want my cheese sandwich."
126
330160
2000
05:32
Which sandwich is he going to take?
127
332160
3000
05:35
Do you think he's going to take that one? Let's see what happens.
128
335160
2000
05:37
Let's see what he does. Here comes Ivan.
129
337160
2000
05:39
And he says, "I want my cheese sandwich."
130
339160
3000
05:42
And he takes this one.
131
342160
2000
05:44
Uh-oh. Why did he take that one?
132
344160
3000
05:47
Child: His was on the grass.
133
347160
4000
05:51
So the three-year-old does two things differently.
134
351160
3000
05:54
First, he predicts Ivan will take the sandwich
135
354160
3000
05:57
that's really his.
136
357160
2000
05:59
And second, when he sees Ivan taking the sandwich where he left his,
137
359160
4000
06:03
where we would say he's taking that one because he thinks it's his,
138
363160
3000
06:06
the three-year-old comes up with another explanation:
139
366160
3000
06:09
He's not taking his own sandwich because he doesn't want it,
140
369160
2000
06:11
because now it's dirty, on the ground.
141
371160
2000
06:13
So that's why he's taking the other sandwich.
142
373160
2000
06:15
Now of course, development doesn't end at five.
143
375160
4000
06:19
And we can see the continuation of this process
144
379160
2000
06:21
of learning to think about other people's thoughts
145
381160
2000
06:23
by upping the ante
146
383160
2000
06:25
and asking children now, not for an action prediction,
147
385160
3000
06:28
but for a moral judgment.
148
388160
2000
06:30
So first I'm going to show you the three-year-old again.
149
390160
2000
06:32
RS.: So is Ivan being mean and naughty for taking Joshua's sandwich?
150
392160
3000
06:35
Child: Yeah.
151
395160
1000
06:36
RS: Should Ivan get in trouble for taking Joshua's sandwich?
152
396160
3000
06:39
Child: Yeah.
153
399160
2000
06:41
So it's maybe not surprising he thinks it was mean of Ivan
154
401160
2000
06:43
to take Joshua's sandwich,
155
403160
2000
06:45
since he thinks Ivan only took Joshua's sandwich
156
405160
2000
06:47
to avoid having to eat his own dirty sandwich.
157
407160
3000
06:50
But now I'm going to show you the five-year-old.
158
410160
2000
06:52
Remember the five-year-old completely understood
159
412160
2000
06:54
why Ivan took Joshua's sandwich.
160
414160
2000
06:56
RS: Was Ivan being mean and naughty
161
416160
2000
06:58
for taking Joshua's sandwich?
162
418160
2000
07:00
Child: Um, yeah.
163
420160
2000
07:02
And so, it is not until age seven
164
422160
2000
07:04
that we get what looks more like an adult response.
165
424160
3000
07:07
RS: Should Ivan get in trouble for taking Joshua's sandwich?
166
427160
3000
07:10
Child: No, because the wind should get in trouble.
167
430160
2000
07:12
He says the wind should get in trouble
168
432160
3000
07:15
for switching the sandwiches.
169
435160
2000
07:17
(Laughter)
170
437160
2000
07:19
And now what we've started to do in my lab
171
439160
2000
07:21
is to put children into the brain scanner
172
441160
2000
07:23
and ask what's going on in their brain
173
443160
3000
07:26
as they develop this ability to think about other people's thoughts.
174
446160
3000
07:29
So the first thing is that in children we see this same brain region, the Right TPJ,
175
449160
4000
07:33
being used while children are thinking about other people.
176
453160
3000
07:36
But it's not quite like the adult brain.
177
456160
2000
07:38
So whereas in the adults, as I told you,
178
458160
2000
07:40
this brain region is almost completely specialized --
179
460160
3000
07:43
it does almost nothing else except for thinking about other people's thoughts --
180
463160
3000
07:46
in children it's much less so,
181
466160
2000
07:48
when they are age five to eight,
182
468160
2000
07:50
the age range of the children I just showed you.
183
470160
2000
07:52
And actually if we even look at eight to 11-year-olds,
184
472160
3000
07:55
getting into early adolescence,
185
475160
2000
07:57
they still don't have quite an adult-like brain region.
186
477160
3000
08:00
And so, what we can see is that over the course of childhood
187
480160
3000
08:03
and even into adolescence,
188
483160
2000
08:05
both the cognitive system,
189
485160
2000
08:07
our mind's ability to think about other minds,
190
487160
2000
08:09
and the brain system that supports it
191
489160
2000
08:11
are continuing, slowly, to develop.
192
491160
3000
08:14
But of course, as you're probably aware,
193
494160
2000
08:16
even in adulthood,
194
496160
2000
08:18
people differ from one another in how good they are
195
498160
2000
08:20
at thinking of other minds, how often they do it
196
500160
2000
08:22
and how accurately.
197
502160
2000
08:24
And so what we wanted to know was, could differences among adults
198
504160
3000
08:27
in how they think about other people's thoughts
199
507160
2000
08:29
be explained in terms of differences in this brain region?
200
509160
3000
08:32
So, the first thing that we did is we gave adults a version
201
512160
3000
08:35
of the pirate problem that we gave to the kids.
202
515160
2000
08:37
And I'm going to give that to you now.
203
517160
2000
08:39
So Grace and her friend are on a tour of a chemical factory,
204
519160
3000
08:42
and they take a break for coffee.
205
522160
2000
08:44
And Grace's friend asks for some sugar in her coffee.
206
524160
3000
08:47
Grace goes to make the coffee
207
527160
3000
08:50
and finds by the coffee a pot
208
530160
2000
08:52
containing a white powder, which is sugar.
209
532160
3000
08:55
But the powder is labeled "Deadly Poison,"
210
535160
3000
08:58
so Grace thinks that the powder is a deadly poison.
211
538160
3000
09:01
And she puts it in her friend's coffee.
212
541160
2000
09:03
And her friend drinks the coffee, and is fine.
213
543160
3000
09:06
How many people think it was morally permissible
214
546160
2000
09:08
for Grace to put the powder in the coffee?
215
548160
4000
09:12
Okay. Good. (Laughter)
216
552160
3000
09:15
So we ask people, how much should Grace be blamed
217
555160
3000
09:18
in this case, which we call a failed attempt to harm?
218
558160
2000
09:20
And we can compare that to another case,
219
560160
2000
09:22
where everything in the real world is the same.
220
562160
2000
09:24
The powder is still sugar, but what's different is what Grace thinks.
221
564160
3000
09:27
Now she thinks the powder is sugar.
222
567160
3000
09:30
And perhaps unsurprisingly, if Grace thinks the powder is sugar
223
570160
3000
09:33
and puts it in her friend's coffee,
224
573160
2000
09:35
people say she deserves no blame at all.
225
575160
2000
09:37
Whereas if she thinks the powder was poison, even though it's really sugar,
226
577160
4000
09:41
now people say she deserves a lot of blame,
227
581160
3000
09:44
even though what happened in the real world was exactly the same.
228
584160
3000
09:47
And in fact, they say she deserves more blame
229
587160
2000
09:49
in this case, the failed attempt to harm,
230
589160
2000
09:51
than in another case,
231
591160
2000
09:53
which we call an accident.
232
593160
2000
09:55
Where Grace thought the powder was sugar,
233
595160
2000
09:57
because it was labeled "sugar" and by the coffee machine,
234
597160
2000
09:59
but actually the powder was poison.
235
599160
2000
10:01
So even though when the powder was poison,
236
601160
3000
10:04
the friend drank the coffee and died,
237
604160
3000
10:07
people say Grace deserves less blame in that case,
238
607160
3000
10:10
when she innocently thought it was sugar,
239
610160
2000
10:12
than in the other case, where she thought it was poison
240
612160
2000
10:14
and no harm occurred.
241
614160
3000
10:17
People, though, disagree a little bit
242
617160
2000
10:19
about exactly how much blame Grace should get
243
619160
2000
10:21
in the accident case.
244
621160
2000
10:23
Some people think she should deserve more blame,
245
623160
2000
10:25
and other people less.
246
625160
2000
10:27
And what I'm going to show you is what happened when we look inside
247
627160
2000
10:29
the brains of people while they're making that judgment.
248
629160
2000
10:31
So what I'm showing you, from left to right,
249
631160
2000
10:33
is how much activity there was in this brain region,
250
633160
3000
10:36
and from top to bottom, how much blame
251
636160
2000
10:38
people said that Grace deserved.
252
638160
2000
10:40
And what you can see is, on the left
253
640160
2000
10:42
when there was very little activity in this brain region,
254
642160
2000
10:44
people paid little attention to her innocent belief
255
644160
3000
10:47
and said she deserved a lot of blame for the accident.
256
647160
3000
10:50
Whereas on the right, where there was a lot of activity,
257
650160
2000
10:52
people paid a lot more attention to her innocent belief,
258
652160
3000
10:55
and said she deserved a lot less blame
259
655160
2000
10:57
for causing the accident.
260
657160
2000
10:59
So that's good, but of course
261
659160
2000
11:01
what we'd rather is have a way to interfere
262
661160
2000
11:03
with function in this brain region,
263
663160
2000
11:05
and see if we could change people's moral judgment.
264
665160
3000
11:08
And we do have such a tool.
265
668160
2000
11:10
It's called Trans-Cranial Magnetic Stimulation,
266
670160
2000
11:12
or TMS.
267
672160
2000
11:14
This is a tool that lets us pass a magnetic pulse
268
674160
2000
11:16
through somebody's skull, into a small region of their brain,
269
676160
4000
11:20
and temporarily disorganize the function of the neurons in that region.
270
680160
4000
11:24
So I'm going to show you a demo of this.
271
684160
2000
11:26
First, I'm going to show you that this is a magnetic pulse.
272
686160
3000
11:29
I'm going to show you what happens when you put a quarter on the machine.
273
689160
3000
11:32
When you hear clicks, we're turning the machine on.
274
692160
4000
11:42
So now I'm going to apply that same pulse to my brain,
275
702160
3000
11:45
to the part of my brain that controls my hand.
276
705160
2000
11:47
So there is no physical force, just a magnetic pulse.
277
707160
3000
11:54
Woman (Video): Ready, Rebecca? RS: Yes.
278
714160
2000
11:57
Okay, so it causes a small involuntary contraction in my hand
279
717160
3000
12:00
by putting a magnetic pulse in my brain.
280
720160
3000
12:03
And we can use that same pulse,
281
723160
2000
12:05
now applied to the RTPJ,
282
725160
2000
12:07
to ask if we can change people's moral judgments.
283
727160
3000
12:10
So these are the judgments I showed you before, people's normal moral judgments.
284
730160
2000
12:12
And then we can apply TMS to the RTPJ
285
732160
3000
12:15
and ask how people's judgments change.
286
735160
2000
12:17
And the first thing is, people can still do this task overall.
287
737160
4000
12:21
So their judgments of the case when everything was fine
288
741160
2000
12:23
remain the same. They say she deserves no blame.
289
743160
3000
12:26
But in the case of a failed attempt to harm,
290
746160
4000
12:30
where Grace thought that it was poison, although it was really sugar,
291
750160
3000
12:33
people now say it was more okay, she deserves less blame
292
753160
3000
12:36
for putting the powder in the coffee.
293
756160
3000
12:39
And in the case of the accident, where she thought that it was sugar,
294
759160
2000
12:41
but it was really poison and so she caused a death,
295
761160
3000
12:44
people say that it was less okay, she deserves more blame.
296
764160
6000
12:50
So what I've told you today is that
297
770160
2000
12:52
people come, actually, especially well equipped
298
772160
4000
12:56
to think about other people's thoughts.
299
776160
2000
12:58
We have a special brain system
300
778160
2000
13:00
that lets us think about what other people are thinking.
301
780160
3000
13:03
This system takes a long time to develop,
302
783160
2000
13:05
slowly throughout the course of childhood and into early adolescence.
303
785160
3000
13:08
And even in adulthood, differences in this brain region
304
788160
3000
13:11
can explain differences among adults
305
791160
2000
13:13
in how we think about and judge other people.
306
793160
3000
13:16
But I want to give the last word back to the novelists,
307
796160
3000
13:19
and to Philip Roth, who ended by saying,
308
799160
3000
13:22
"The fact remains that getting people right
309
802160
2000
13:24
is not what living is all about anyway.
310
804160
2000
13:26
It's getting them wrong that is living.
311
806160
2000
13:28
Getting them wrong and wrong and wrong,
312
808160
3000
13:31
and then on careful reconsideration,
313
811160
2000
13:33
getting them wrong again."
314
813160
2000
13:35
Thank you.
315
815160
2000
13:37
(Applause)
316
817160
10000
13:47
Chris Anderson: So, I have a question. When you start talking about using
317
827160
2000
13:49
magnetic pulses to change people's moral judgments,
318
829160
3000
13:52
that sounds alarming.
319
832160
3000
13:55
(Laughter)
320
835160
1000
13:56
Please tell me that you're not taking phone calls from the Pentagon, say.
321
836160
4000
14:00
RS: I'm not.
322
840160
2000
14:02
I mean, they're calling, but I'm not taking the call.
323
842160
3000
14:05
(Laughter)
324
845160
1000
14:06
CA: They really are calling?
325
846160
2000
14:08
So then seriously,
326
848160
3000
14:11
you must lie awake at night sometimes
327
851160
3000
14:14
wondering where this work leads.
328
854160
2000
14:16
I mean, you're clearly an incredible human being,
329
856160
2000
14:18
but someone could take this knowledge
330
858160
3000
14:21
and in some future
331
861160
2000
14:23
not-torture chamber,
332
863160
2000
14:25
do acts that people here might be worried about.
333
865160
3000
14:28
RS: Yeah, we worry about this.
334
868160
2000
14:30
So, there's a couple of things to say about TMS.
335
870160
3000
14:33
One is that you can't be TMSed without knowing it.
336
873160
2000
14:35
So it's not a surreptitious technology.
337
875160
3000
14:38
It's quite hard, actually, to get those very small changes.
338
878160
3000
14:41
The changes I showed you are impressive to me
339
881160
3000
14:44
because of what they tell us about the function of the brain,
340
884160
2000
14:46
but they're small on the scale
341
886160
2000
14:48
of the moral judgments that we actually make.
342
888160
2000
14:50
And what we changed was not people's
343
890160
2000
14:52
moral judgments when they're deciding what to do,
344
892160
3000
14:55
when they're making action choices.
345
895160
2000
14:57
We changed their ability to judge other people's actions.
346
897160
3000
15:00
And so, I think of what I'm doing not so much as
347
900160
2000
15:02
studying the defendant in a criminal trial,
348
902160
2000
15:04
but studying the jury.
349
904160
2000
15:06
CA: Is your work going to lead to any recommendations
350
906160
3000
15:09
in education, to perhaps bring up
351
909160
3000
15:12
a generation of kids able to make fairer moral judgments?
352
912160
5000
15:17
RS: That's one of the idealistic hopes.
353
917160
3000
15:20
The whole research program here of studying
354
920160
4000
15:24
the distinctive parts of the human brain is brand new.
355
924160
4000
15:28
Until recently, what we knew about the brain
356
928160
2000
15:30
were the things that any other animal's brain could do too,
357
930160
3000
15:33
so we could study it in animal models.
358
933160
2000
15:35
We knew how brains see, and how they control the body
359
935160
2000
15:37
and how they hear and sense.
360
937160
2000
15:39
And the whole project of understanding
361
939160
3000
15:42
how brains do the uniquely human things --
362
942160
2000
15:44
learn language and abstract concepts,
363
944160
3000
15:47
and thinking about other people's thoughts -- that's brand new.
364
947160
2000
15:49
And we don't know yet what the implications will be
365
949160
2000
15:51
of understanding it.
366
951160
2000
15:53
CA: So I've got one last question. There is this thing called
367
953160
2000
15:55
the hard problem of consciousness,
368
955160
2000
15:57
that puzzles a lot of people.
369
957160
2000
15:59
The notion that you can understand
370
959160
3000
16:02
why a brain works, perhaps.
371
962160
2000
16:04
But why does anyone have to feel anything?
372
964160
3000
16:07
Why does it seem to require these beings who sense things
373
967160
3000
16:10
for us to operate?
374
970160
2000
16:12
You're a brilliant young neuroscientist.
375
972160
3000
16:15
I mean, what chances do you think there are
376
975160
2000
16:17
that at some time in your career,
377
977160
2000
16:19
someone, you or someone else,
378
979160
2000
16:21
is going to come up with some paradigm shift
379
981160
2000
16:23
in understanding what seems an impossible problem?
380
983160
4000
16:27
RS: I hope they do. And I think they probably won't.
381
987160
4000
16:31
CA: Why?
382
991160
3000
16:34
RS: It's not called the hard problem of consciousness for nothing.
383
994160
3000
16:37
(Laughter)
384
997160
2000
16:39
CA: That's a great answer. Rebecca Saxe, thank you very much. That was fantastic.
385
999160
3000
16:42
(Applause)
386
1002160
4000
About this website

This site will introduce you to YouTube videos that are useful for learning English. You will see English lessons taught by top-notch teachers from around the world. Double-click on the English subtitles displayed on each video page to play the video from there. The subtitles scroll in sync with the video playback. If you have any comments or requests, please contact us using this contact form.

https://forms.gle/WvT1wiN1qDtmnspy7