Thomas Goetz: It's time to redesign medical data

57,314 views ・ 2011-01-27

TED


Please double-click on the English subtitles below to play the video.

00:15
I'm going to be talking to you
0
15330
2000
00:17
about how we can tap
1
17330
2000
00:19
a really underutilized resource in health care,
2
19330
2000
00:21
which is the patient,
3
21330
2000
00:23
or, as I like to use the scientific term,
4
23330
3000
00:26
people.
5
26330
2000
00:28
Because we are all patients, we are all people.
6
28330
2000
00:30
Even doctors are patients at some point.
7
30330
2000
00:32
So I want to talk about that
8
32330
2000
00:34
as an opportunity
9
34330
2000
00:36
that we really have failed to engage with very well in this country
10
36330
3000
00:39
and, in fact, worldwide.
11
39330
2000
00:41
If you want to get at the big part --
12
41330
2000
00:43
I mean from a public health level, where my training is --
13
43330
3000
00:46
you're looking at behavioral issues.
14
46330
2000
00:48
You're looking at things where people are actually given information,
15
48330
3000
00:51
and they're not following through with it.
16
51330
2000
00:53
It's a problem that manifests itself in diabetes,
17
53330
4000
00:57
obesity, many forms of heart disease,
18
57330
2000
00:59
even some forms of cancer -- when you think of smoking.
19
59330
3000
01:02
Those are all behaviors where people know what they're supposed to do.
20
62330
3000
01:05
They know what they're supposed to be doing,
21
65330
2000
01:07
but they're not doing it.
22
67330
2000
01:09
Now behavior change is something
23
69330
2000
01:11
that is a long-standing problem in medicine.
24
71330
2000
01:13
It goes all the way back to Aristotle.
25
73330
2000
01:15
And doctors hate it, right?
26
75330
2000
01:17
I mean, they complain about it all the time.
27
77330
2000
01:19
We talk about it in terms of engagement, or non-compliance.
28
79330
3000
01:22
When people don't take their pills,
29
82330
2000
01:24
when people don't follow doctors' orders --
30
84330
2000
01:26
these are behavior problems.
31
86330
2000
01:28
But for as much as clinical medicine
32
88330
2000
01:30
agonizes over behavior change,
33
90330
2000
01:32
there's not a lot of work done
34
92330
2000
01:34
in terms of trying to fix that problem.
35
94330
3000
01:37
So the crux of it
36
97330
2000
01:39
comes down to this notion of decision-making --
37
99330
2000
01:41
giving information to people in a form
38
101330
2000
01:43
that doesn't just educate them
39
103330
2000
01:45
or inform them,
40
105330
2000
01:47
but actually leads them to make better decisions,
41
107330
2000
01:49
better choices in their lives.
42
109330
2000
01:51
One part of medicine, though,
43
111330
2000
01:53
has faced the problem of behavior change pretty well,
44
113330
4000
01:57
and that's dentistry.
45
117330
2000
01:59
Dentistry might seem -- and I think it is --
46
119330
2000
02:01
many dentists would have to acknowledge
47
121330
2000
02:03
it's somewhat of a mundane backwater of medicine.
48
123330
2000
02:05
Not a lot of cool, sexy stuff happening in dentistry.
49
125330
3000
02:08
But they have really taken this problem of behavior change
50
128330
3000
02:11
and solved it.
51
131330
2000
02:13
It's the one great preventive health success
52
133330
2000
02:15
we have in our health care system.
53
135330
2000
02:17
People brush and floss their teeth.
54
137330
2000
02:19
They don't do it as much as they should, but they do it.
55
139330
3000
02:22
So I'm going to talk about one experiment
56
142330
2000
02:24
that a few dentists in Connecticut
57
144330
2000
02:26
cooked up about 30 years ago.
58
146330
2000
02:28
So this is an old experiment, but it's a really good one,
59
148330
2000
02:30
because it was very simple,
60
150330
2000
02:32
so it's an easy story to tell.
61
152330
2000
02:34
So these Connecticut dentists decided
62
154330
2000
02:36
that they wanted to get people to brush their teeth and floss their teeth more often,
63
156330
3000
02:39
and they were going to use one variable:
64
159330
2000
02:41
they wanted to scare them.
65
161330
2000
02:43
They wanted to tell them how bad it would be
66
163330
3000
02:46
if they didn't brush and floss their teeth.
67
166330
2000
02:48
They had a big patient population.
68
168330
3000
02:51
They divided them up into two groups.
69
171330
2000
02:53
They had a low-fear population,
70
173330
2000
02:55
where they basically gave them a 13-minute presentation,
71
175330
2000
02:57
all based in science,
72
177330
2000
02:59
but told them that, if you didn't brush and floss your teeth,
73
179330
3000
03:02
you could get gum disease. If you get gum disease, you will lose your teeth,
74
182330
3000
03:05
but you'll get dentures, and it won't be that bad.
75
185330
2000
03:07
So that was the low-fear group.
76
187330
2000
03:09
The high-fear group, they laid it on really thick.
77
189330
3000
03:12
They showed bloody gums.
78
192330
2000
03:14
They showed puss oozing out from between their teeth.
79
194330
3000
03:17
They told them that their teeth were going to fall out.
80
197330
2000
03:19
They said that they could have infections
81
199330
2000
03:21
that would spread from their jaws to other parts of their bodies,
82
201330
3000
03:24
and ultimately, yes, they would lose their teeth.
83
204330
2000
03:26
They would get dentures, and if you got dentures,
84
206330
2000
03:28
you weren't going to be able to eat corn-on-the-cob,
85
208330
2000
03:30
you weren't going to be able to eat apples,
86
210330
2000
03:32
you weren't going to be able to eat steak.
87
212330
2000
03:34
You'll eat mush for the rest of your life.
88
214330
2000
03:36
So go brush and floss your teeth.
89
216330
3000
03:39
That was the message. That was the experiment.
90
219330
2000
03:41
Now they measured one other variable.
91
221330
2000
03:43
They wanted to capture one other variable,
92
223330
2000
03:45
which was the patients' sense of efficacy.
93
225330
3000
03:48
This was the notion of whether the patients felt
94
228330
2000
03:50
that they actually would go ahead and brush and floss their teeth.
95
230330
3000
03:53
So they asked them at the beginning,
96
233330
2000
03:55
"Do you think you'll actually be able to stick with this program?"
97
235330
2000
03:57
And the people who said, "Yeah, yeah. I'm pretty good about that,"
98
237330
2000
03:59
they were characterized as high efficacy,
99
239330
2000
04:01
and the people who said,
100
241330
2000
04:03
"Eh, I never get around to brushing and flossing as much as I should,"
101
243330
2000
04:05
they were characterized as low efficacy.
102
245330
2000
04:07
So the upshot was this.
103
247330
3000
04:10
The upshot of this experiment
104
250330
2000
04:12
was that fear was not really a primary driver
105
252330
3000
04:15
of the behavior at all.
106
255330
2000
04:17
The people who brushed and flossed their teeth
107
257330
2000
04:19
were not necessarily the people
108
259330
2000
04:21
who were really scared about what would happen --
109
261330
2000
04:23
it's the people who simply felt that they had the capacity
110
263330
3000
04:26
to change their behavior.
111
266330
2000
04:28
So fear showed up as not really the driver.
112
268330
3000
04:31
It was the sense of efficacy.
113
271330
3000
04:34
So I want to isolate this,
114
274330
2000
04:36
because it was a great observation --
115
276330
2000
04:38
30 years ago, right, 30 years ago --
116
278330
2000
04:40
and it's one that's laid fallow in research.
117
280330
3000
04:43
It was a notion that really came out
118
283330
2000
04:45
of Albert Bandura's work,
119
285330
2000
04:47
who studied whether
120
287330
2000
04:49
people could get a sense of empowerment.
121
289330
3000
04:52
The notion of efficacy basically boils down to one -- that
122
292330
3000
04:55
if somebody believes that they have the capacity to change their behavior.
123
295330
3000
04:58
In health care terms, you could characterize this
124
298330
3000
05:01
as whether or not somebody feels
125
301330
2000
05:03
that they see a path towards better health,
126
303330
2000
05:05
that they can actually see their way towards getting better health,
127
305330
2000
05:07
and that's a very important notion.
128
307330
2000
05:09
It's an amazing notion.
129
309330
2000
05:11
We don't really know how to manipulate it, though, that well.
130
311330
3000
05:14
Except, maybe we do.
131
314330
3000
05:17
So fear doesn't work, right? Fear doesn't work.
132
317330
2000
05:19
And this is a great example
133
319330
2000
05:21
of how we haven't learned that lesson at all.
134
321330
3000
05:24
This is a campaign from the American Diabetes Association.
135
324330
3000
05:27
This is still the way we're communicating messages about health.
136
327330
3000
05:30
I mean, I showed my three-year-old this slide last night,
137
330330
3000
05:33
and he's like, "Papa, why is an ambulance in these people's homes?"
138
333330
4000
05:37
And I had to explain, "They're trying to scare people."
139
337330
3000
05:40
And I don't know if it works.
140
340330
2000
05:42
Now here's what does work:
141
342330
2000
05:44
personalized information works.
142
344330
2000
05:46
Again, Bandura recognized this
143
346330
2000
05:48
years ago, decades ago.
144
348330
2000
05:50
When you give people specific information
145
350330
2000
05:52
about their health, where they stand,
146
352330
2000
05:54
and where they want to get to, where they might get to,
147
354330
2000
05:56
that path, that notion of a path --
148
356330
2000
05:58
that tends to work for behavior change.
149
358330
2000
06:00
So let me just spool it out a little bit.
150
360330
2000
06:02
So you start with personalized data, personalized information
151
362330
3000
06:05
that comes from an individual,
152
365330
2000
06:07
and then you need to connect it to their lives.
153
367330
3000
06:10
You need to connect it to their lives,
154
370330
2000
06:12
hopefully not in a fear-based way, but one that they understand.
155
372330
2000
06:14
Okay, I know where I sit. I know where I'm situated.
156
374330
3000
06:17
And that doesn't just work for me in terms of abstract numbers --
157
377330
3000
06:20
this overload of health information
158
380330
2000
06:22
that we're inundated with.
159
382330
2000
06:24
But it actually hits home.
160
384330
2000
06:26
It's not just hitting us in our heads; it's hitting us in our hearts.
161
386330
2000
06:28
There's an emotional connection to information
162
388330
2000
06:30
because it's from us.
163
390330
2000
06:32
That information then needs to be connected to choices,
164
392330
3000
06:35
needs to be connected to a range of options,
165
395330
2000
06:37
directions that we might go to --
166
397330
2000
06:39
trade-offs, benefits.
167
399330
2000
06:41
Finally, we need to be presented with a clear point of action.
168
401330
3000
06:44
We need to connect the information
169
404330
2000
06:46
always with the action,
170
406330
2000
06:48
and then that action feeds back
171
408330
2000
06:50
into different information,
172
410330
2000
06:52
and it creates, of course, a feedback loop.
173
412330
2000
06:54
Now this is a very well-observed and well-established notion
174
414330
3000
06:57
for behavior change.
175
417330
2000
06:59
But the problem is that things -- in the upper-right corner there --
176
419330
3000
07:02
personalized data, it's been pretty hard to come by.
177
422330
2000
07:04
It's a difficult and expensive commodity,
178
424330
3000
07:07
until now.
179
427330
2000
07:09
So I'm going to give you an example, a very simple example of how this works.
180
429330
3000
07:12
So we've all seen these. These are the "your speed limit" signs.
181
432330
3000
07:15
You've seen them all around,
182
435330
2000
07:17
especially these days as radars are cheaper.
183
437330
2000
07:19
And here's how they work in the feedback loop.
184
439330
2000
07:21
So you start with the personalized data
185
441330
2000
07:23
where the speed limit on the road that you are at that point
186
443330
2000
07:25
is 25,
187
445330
2000
07:27
and, of course, you're going faster than that.
188
447330
2000
07:29
We always are. We're always going above the speed limit.
189
449330
3000
07:32
The choice in this case is pretty simple.
190
452330
2000
07:34
We either keep going fast, or we slow down.
191
454330
2000
07:36
We should probably slow down,
192
456330
2000
07:38
and that point of action is probably now.
193
458330
2000
07:40
We should take our foot off the pedal right now,
194
460330
3000
07:43
and generally we do. These things are shown to be pretty effective
195
463330
3000
07:46
in terms of getting people to slow down.
196
466330
2000
07:48
They reduce speeds by about five to 10 percent.
197
468330
2000
07:50
They last for about five miles,
198
470330
2000
07:52
in which case we put our foot back on the pedal.
199
472330
2000
07:54
But it works, and it even has some health repercussions.
200
474330
2000
07:56
Your blood pressure might drop a little bit.
201
476330
2000
07:58
Maybe there's fewer accidents, so there's public health benefits.
202
478330
3000
08:01
But by and large, this is a feedback loop
203
481330
2000
08:03
that's so nifty and too rare.
204
483330
3000
08:06
Because in health care, most health care,
205
486330
2000
08:08
the data is very removed from the action.
206
488330
3000
08:11
It's very difficult to line things up so neatly.
207
491330
3000
08:14
But we have an opportunity.
208
494330
2000
08:16
So I want to talk about, I want to shift now to think about
209
496330
2000
08:18
how we deliver health information in this country,
210
498330
2000
08:20
how we actually get information.
211
500330
3000
08:23
This is a pharmaceutical ad.
212
503330
3000
08:26
Actually, it's a spoof. It's not a real pharmaceutical ad.
213
506330
2000
08:28
Nobody's had the brilliant idea
214
508330
2000
08:30
of calling their drug Havidol quite yet.
215
510330
3000
08:34
But it looks completely right.
216
514330
2000
08:36
So it's exactly the way we get
217
516330
2000
08:38
health information and pharmaceutical information,
218
518330
3000
08:41
and it just sounds perfect.
219
521330
2000
08:43
And then we turn the page of the magazine,
220
523330
2000
08:45
and we see this --
221
525330
3000
08:48
now this is the page the FDA requires pharmaceutical companies
222
528330
3000
08:51
to put into their ads, or to follow their ads,
223
531330
3000
08:54
and to me, this is one of the most cynical exercises in medicine.
224
534330
4000
08:58
Because we know.
225
538330
2000
09:00
Who among us would actually say that people read this?
226
540330
2000
09:02
And who among us would actually say
227
542330
2000
09:04
that people who do try to read this
228
544330
2000
09:06
actually get anything out of it?
229
546330
2000
09:08
This is a bankrupt effort
230
548330
2000
09:10
at communicating health information.
231
550330
3000
09:13
There is no good faith in this.
232
553330
2000
09:15
So this is a different approach.
233
555330
2000
09:17
This is an approach that has been developed
234
557330
3000
09:20
by a couple researchers at Dartmouth Medical School,
235
560330
3000
09:23
Lisa Schwartz and Steven Woloshin.
236
563330
2000
09:25
And they created this thing called the "drug facts box."
237
565330
3000
09:28
They took inspiration from, of all things,
238
568330
2000
09:30
Cap'n Crunch.
239
570330
2000
09:32
They went to the nutritional information box
240
572330
3000
09:35
and saw that what works for cereal, works for our food,
241
575330
3000
09:38
actually helps people understand what's in their food.
242
578330
3000
09:42
God forbid we should use that same standard
243
582330
2000
09:44
that we make Cap'n Crunch live by
244
584330
2000
09:46
and bring it to drug companies.
245
586330
3000
09:49
So let me just walk through this quickly.
246
589330
2000
09:51
It says very clearly what the drug is for, specifically who it is good for,
247
591330
3000
09:54
so you can start to personalize your understanding
248
594330
2000
09:56
of whether the information is relevant to you
249
596330
2000
09:58
or whether the drug is relevant to you.
250
598330
2000
10:00
You can understand exactly what the benefits are.
251
600330
3000
10:03
It isn't this kind of vague promise that it's going to work no matter what,
252
603330
3000
10:06
but you get the statistics for how effective it is.
253
606330
3000
10:09
And finally, you understand what those choices are.
254
609330
3000
10:12
You can start to unpack the choices involved
255
612330
2000
10:14
because of the side effects.
256
614330
2000
10:16
Every time you take a drug, you're walking into a possible side effect.
257
616330
3000
10:19
So it spells those out in very clean terms,
258
619330
2000
10:21
and that works.
259
621330
2000
10:23
So I love this. I love that drug facts box.
260
623330
2000
10:25
And so I was thinking about,
261
625330
2000
10:27
what's an opportunity that I could have
262
627330
2000
10:29
to help people understand information?
263
629330
3000
10:32
What's another latent body of information that's out there
264
632330
4000
10:36
that people are really not putting to use?
265
636330
3000
10:39
And so I came up with this: lab test results.
266
639330
3000
10:42
Blood test results are this great source of information.
267
642330
3000
10:45
They're packed with information.
268
645330
2000
10:47
They're just not for us. They're not for people. They're not for patients.
269
647330
3000
10:50
They go right to doctors.
270
650330
2000
10:52
And God forbid -- I think many doctors, if you really asked them,
271
652330
3000
10:55
they don't really understand all this stuff either.
272
655330
3000
10:58
This is the worst presented information.
273
658330
3000
11:01
You ask Tufte, and he would say,
274
661330
3000
11:04
"Yes, this is the absolute worst presentation of information possible."
275
664330
3000
11:07
What we did at Wired
276
667330
2000
11:09
was we went, and I got our graphic design department
277
669330
2000
11:11
to re-imagine these lab reports.
278
671330
2000
11:13
So that's what I want to walk you through.
279
673330
2000
11:15
So this is the general blood work before,
280
675330
3000
11:18
and this is the after, this is what we came up with.
281
678330
2000
11:20
The after takes what was four pages --
282
680330
2000
11:22
that previous slide was actually
283
682330
2000
11:24
the first of four pages of data
284
684330
2000
11:26
that's just the general blood work.
285
686330
2000
11:28
It goes on and on and on, all these values, all these numbers you don't know.
286
688330
3000
11:31
This is our one-page summary.
287
691330
3000
11:34
We use the notion of color.
288
694330
2000
11:36
It's an amazing notion that color could be used.
289
696330
3000
11:39
So on the top-level you have your overall results,
290
699330
3000
11:42
the things that might jump out at you from the fine print.
291
702330
3000
11:45
Then you can drill down
292
705330
2000
11:47
and understand how actually we put your level in context,
293
707330
3000
11:50
and we use color to illustrate
294
710330
2000
11:52
exactly where your value falls.
295
712330
2000
11:54
In this case, this patient is slightly at risk of diabetes
296
714330
3000
11:57
because of their glucose level.
297
717330
2000
11:59
Likewise, you can go over your lipids
298
719330
2000
12:01
and, again, understand what your overall cholesterol level is
299
721330
3000
12:04
and then break down into the HDL and the LDL if you so choose.
300
724330
3000
12:07
But again, always using color
301
727330
2000
12:09
and personalized proximity
302
729330
2000
12:11
to that information.
303
731330
2000
12:13
All those other values,
304
733330
2000
12:15
all those pages and pages of values that are full of nothing,
305
735330
2000
12:17
we summarize.
306
737330
2000
12:19
We tell you that you're okay, you're normal.
307
739330
2000
12:21
But you don't have to wade through it. You don't have to go through the junk.
308
741330
3000
12:24
And then we do two other very important things
309
744330
2000
12:26
that kind of help fill in this feedback loop:
310
746330
2000
12:28
we help people understand in a little more detail
311
748330
2000
12:30
what these values are and what they might indicate.
312
750330
3000
12:33
And then we go a further step -- we tell them what they can do.
313
753330
3000
12:36
We give them some insight
314
756330
2000
12:38
into what choices they can make, what actions they can take.
315
758330
3000
12:41
So that's our general blood work test.
316
761330
3000
12:44
Then we went to CRP test.
317
764330
2000
12:46
In this case, it's a sin of omission.
318
766330
2000
12:48
They have this huge amount of space,
319
768330
2000
12:50
and they don't use it for anything, so we do.
320
770330
2000
12:52
Now the CRP test is often done
321
772330
2000
12:54
following a cholesterol test,
322
774330
2000
12:56
or in conjunction with a cholesterol test.
323
776330
2000
12:58
So we take the bold step
324
778330
2000
13:00
of putting the cholesterol information on the same page,
325
780330
3000
13:03
which is the way the doctor is going to evaluate it.
326
783330
2000
13:05
So we thought the patient might actually want to know the context as well.
327
785330
3000
13:08
It's a protein that shows up
328
788330
2000
13:10
when your blood vessels might be inflamed,
329
790330
2000
13:12
which might be a risk for heart disease.
330
792330
2000
13:14
What you're actually measuring
331
794330
2000
13:16
is spelled out in clean language.
332
796330
2000
13:18
Then we use the information
333
798330
2000
13:20
that's already in the lab report.
334
800330
2000
13:22
We use the person's age and their gender
335
802330
2000
13:24
to start to fill in the personalized risks.
336
804330
3000
13:27
So we start to use the data we have
337
807330
2000
13:29
to run a very simple calculation
338
809330
2000
13:31
that's on all sorts of online calculators
339
811330
2000
13:33
to get a sense of what the actual risk is.
340
813330
3000
13:36
The last one I'll show you is a PSA test.
341
816330
2000
13:38
Here's the before, and here's the after.
342
818330
3000
13:41
Now a lot of our effort on this one --
343
821330
2000
13:43
as many of you probably know,
344
823330
2000
13:45
a PSA test is a very controversial test.
345
825330
2000
13:47
It's used to test for prostate cancer,
346
827330
2000
13:49
but there are all sorts of reasons
347
829330
2000
13:51
why your prostate might be enlarged.
348
831330
2000
13:53
And so we spent a good deal of our time
349
833330
2000
13:55
indicating that.
350
835330
2000
13:57
We again personalized the risks.
351
837330
2000
13:59
So this patient is in their 50s,
352
839330
2000
14:01
so we can actually give them a very precise estimate
353
841330
2000
14:03
of what their risk for prostate cancer is.
354
843330
2000
14:05
In this case it's about 25 percent, based on that.
355
845330
3000
14:08
And then again, the follow-up actions.
356
848330
3000
14:11
So our cost for this was less than 10,000 dollars, all right.
357
851330
3000
14:14
That's what Wired magazine spent on this.
358
854330
3000
14:17
Why is Wired magazine doing this?
359
857330
2000
14:19
(Laughter)
360
859330
3000
14:22
Quest Diagnostics and LabCorp,
361
862330
2000
14:24
the two largest lab testing companies --
362
864330
3000
14:27
last year, they made profits of over 700 million dollars
363
867330
3000
14:30
and over 500 million dollars respectively.
364
870330
3000
14:33
Now this is not a problem of resources;
365
873330
2000
14:35
this is a problem of incentives.
366
875330
3000
14:38
We need to recognize that the target of this information
367
878330
3000
14:41
should not be the doctor, should not be the insurance company.
368
881330
3000
14:44
It should be the patient.
369
884330
2000
14:46
It's the person who actually, in the end,
370
886330
2000
14:48
is going to be having to change their lives
371
888330
2000
14:50
and then start adopting new behaviors.
372
890330
2000
14:52
This is information that is incredibly powerful.
373
892330
2000
14:54
It's an incredibly powerful catalyst to change.
374
894330
3000
14:57
But we're not using it. It's just sitting there.
375
897330
2000
14:59
It's being lost.
376
899330
2000
15:01
So I want to just offer four questions
377
901330
2000
15:03
that every patient should ask,
378
903330
2000
15:05
because I don't actually expect people
379
905330
2000
15:07
to start developing these lab test reports.
380
907330
2000
15:09
But you can create your own feedback loop.
381
909330
2000
15:11
Anybody can create their feedback loop by asking these simple questions:
382
911330
3000
15:14
Can I have my results?
383
914330
2000
15:16
And the only acceptable answer is --
384
916330
2000
15:18
(Audience: Yes.) -- yes.
385
918330
2000
15:20
What does this mean? Help me understand what the data is.
386
920330
2000
15:22
What are my options? What choices are now on the table?
387
922330
3000
15:25
And then, what's next?
388
925330
2000
15:27
How do I integrate this information
389
927330
2000
15:29
into the longer course of my life?
390
929330
2000
15:32
So I want to wind up by just showing
391
932330
2000
15:34
that people have the capacity to understand this information.
392
934330
2000
15:36
This is not beyond the grasp of ordinary people.
393
936330
3000
15:39
You do not need to have the education level of people in this room.
394
939330
3000
15:42
Ordinary people are capable of understanding this information,
395
942330
3000
15:45
if we only go to the effort of presenting it to them
396
945330
3000
15:48
in a form that they can engage with.
397
948330
2000
15:50
And engagement is essential here,
398
950330
2000
15:52
because it's not just giving them information;
399
952330
2000
15:54
it's giving them an opportunity to act.
400
954330
2000
15:56
That's what engagement is. It's different from compliance.
401
956330
2000
15:58
It works totally different from the way we talk about behavior
402
958330
3000
16:01
in medicine today.
403
961330
2000
16:03
And this information is out there.
404
963330
2000
16:05
I've been talking today about latent information,
405
965330
2000
16:07
all this information that exists in the system
406
967330
2000
16:09
that we're not putting to use.
407
969330
2000
16:11
But there are all sorts of other bodies of information
408
971330
2000
16:13
that are coming online,
409
973330
2000
16:15
and we need to recognize the capacity of this information
410
975330
3000
16:18
to engage people, to help people
411
978330
2000
16:20
and to change the course of their lives.
412
980330
2000
16:22
Thank you very much.
413
982330
2000
16:24
(Applause)
414
984330
3000
About this website

This site will introduce you to YouTube videos that are useful for learning English. You will see English lessons taught by top-notch teachers from around the world. Double-click on the English subtitles displayed on each video page to play the video from there. The subtitles scroll in sync with the video playback. If you have any comments or requests, please contact us using this contact form.

https://forms.gle/WvT1wiN1qDtmnspy7