Are You Really As Good at Something As You Think? | Robin Kramer | TED

94,729 views ・ 2023-11-16

TED


Please double-click on the English subtitles below to play the video.

00:04
I don't mean to brag,
0
4300
1760
00:06
but there are lots of things that I'm pretty average at.
1
6060
3000
00:09
From playing table tennis,
2
9100
1400
00:10
cooking risotto, finding countries on a map,
3
10500
2560
00:13
just to name a few.
4
13100
1400
00:14
Now, in our everyday lives,
5
14540
1440
00:16
we're not typically assessed on our skills and abilities,
6
16020
2760
00:18
so we're forced to rely on our own judgments.
7
18820
2400
00:21
I may think I'm pretty decent with Italian cuisine,
8
21260
3080
00:24
but how accurate is my assessment?
9
24380
2520
00:26
Now, what we’re talking about here is metacognition:
10
26940
3080
00:30
our insight into our own thought processes.
11
30020
2920
00:33
If I have good metacognitive insight,
12
33620
2320
00:35
then how good I think I am at a particular task
13
35940
2880
00:38
should line up pretty well with how good I actually am.
14
38860
3320
00:42
Of course, in the real world this is often not the case.
15
42180
2840
00:45
And indeed, we probably all know someone
16
45780
2160
00:47
who thinks they're great at navigating maps,
17
47980
2760
00:50
when in fact the reality is often the opposite.
18
50780
2920
00:53
Not to name any names, of course, but still.
19
53740
2960
00:57
Perhaps you think this applies to other people
20
57260
2400
00:59
and that you, yourself, wouldn't make this sort of mistake.
21
59700
3080
01:02
So let's try a quick experiment.
22
62780
2600
01:05
I want you to think about how you would rate yourself
23
65380
2680
01:08
in terms of your driving ability.
24
68100
1960
01:10
Would you rate yourself as below average, average
25
70060
2480
01:12
or perhaps even above average?
26
72580
2360
01:15
So most people rate themselves as above average,
27
75380
3560
01:18
which, of course, is mathematically impossible,
28
78980
2320
01:21
and something that we call the "better than average" effect.
29
81300
3120
01:24
This is just one of a number of cognitive biases that we see
30
84460
3160
01:27
when people judge their own abilities.
31
87620
2200
01:30
Today, I'm going to focus on a related bias,
32
90180
2560
01:32
the Dunning-Kruger effect.
33
92740
1880
01:34
So back in 1999,
34
94660
1680
01:36
two psychologists at Cornell University, Dunning and Kruger,
35
96380
3720
01:40
described the mistakes people make when estimating their own abilities.
36
100100
4480
01:44
So if we take a sample of people and we divide them into four groups
37
104580
3320
01:47
based on their scores on a test,
38
107940
1840
01:49
and order those groups from lowest to highest.
39
109820
2400
01:52
If we plot those scores on a graph along with their self-estimates,
40
112260
3880
01:56
so how well they thought they did on the test,
41
116180
2680
01:58
this is the pattern that we see.
42
118900
1840
02:00
So the red line is a steep slope representing their actual scores.
43
120780
4800
02:05
As it must be, since we ordered the groups
44
125580
2040
02:07
based on their scores in the first place.
45
127620
2120
02:09
Now what's interesting is the blue shallower line.
46
129780
3320
02:13
This represents their self-estimates.
47
133140
2400
02:15
So, how good they thought they did on the test.
48
135580
2520
02:18
Now the Dunning-Kruger effect describes how the weakest performers
49
138540
4680
02:23
significantly overestimate their performance,
50
143220
2720
02:25
shown here in the green oval.
51
145980
1920
02:28
The explanation for this, according to Dunning and Kruger,
52
148500
3480
02:31
is that insight and ability rely on the same thing.
53
151980
3840
02:35
So if I'm poor at a task,
54
155860
1920
02:37
I also lack the metacognitive insight to accurately assess my ability.
55
157820
4800
02:43
Now this pattern has been seen again and again
56
163540
2320
02:45
across a number of domains,
57
165860
1440
02:47
from driving skill to exam-taking, even chess-playing.
58
167340
3440
02:51
However, in recent years,
59
171620
1600
02:53
a number of criticisms have been leveled at this approach,
60
173220
3000
02:56
and we now have reason to believe
61
176260
1600
02:57
that this pattern results is virtually unavoidable.
62
177900
3000
03:02
One reason for this is the statistical effect,
63
182460
3000
03:05
regression to the mean.
64
185460
1520
03:07
Now this is something that comes about
65
187380
1840
03:09
when we have two measures that are related but not perfectly so.
66
189220
3720
03:13
So imagine we have a sample of people
67
193540
1840
03:15
and we measure their heights and their weights.
68
195420
2440
03:17
Now height and weight are related,
69
197900
1880
03:19
tall people are typically heavier,
70
199820
2600
03:22
but the relationship is far from perfect.
71
202420
2480
03:24
So unlike in the figure at the top here,
72
204940
3240
03:28
the shortest people in red won't all be the lightest people.
73
208220
4000
03:32
Some of them will be overweight or particularly muscular, for example.
74
212220
3520
03:35
Similarly at the top end,
75
215780
1640
03:37
the tallest people in blue won't all be the heaviest people.
76
217460
3800
03:41
Some of them will be underweight, and so on.
77
221260
2600
03:43
Now as a result, on average,
78
223900
2080
03:46
the shortest people will rank higher for weight than they do for height,
79
226020
4200
03:50
and the tallest people will rank lower for weight than they do for height
80
230220
3880
03:54
producing this blue line here
81
234140
1800
03:55
and the crossover pattern you're now becoming familiar with.
82
235980
3040
03:59
Now, some people might put forward a spurious explanation
83
239420
3120
04:02
for why short people are relatively overweight
84
242540
2840
04:05
or tall people relatively underweight,
85
245420
2120
04:07
when in fact no explanation is needed.
86
247580
2400
04:10
Perhaps more compelling a reason to doubt the Dunning-Kruger effect
87
250700
4240
04:14
is that we can produce the same pattern in our data
88
254980
2640
04:17
when our data is entirely meaningless.
89
257660
2640
04:20
So if we collect people's test scores
90
260340
2720
04:23
along with their self-estimates of those scores,
91
263100
2640
04:25
but then we shuffle those self-estimates
92
265740
2520
04:28
and then analyze as before,
93
268260
2000
04:30
then we still find that same pattern in the data.
94
270260
2760
04:33
Of course, any effect that we can find
95
273420
2160
04:35
with shuffled or randomized data
96
275580
2480
04:38
is one that we should surely be suspicious of.
97
278100
2320
04:41
So, given these and other issues with the Dunning-Kruger approach,
98
281140
4240
04:45
I was saddened and disappointed
99
285420
1920
04:47
and, frankly, a little annoyed to discover
100
287380
2600
04:49
that the same approach was now being applied
101
289980
2320
04:52
in my field of expertise,
102
292340
1480
04:53
which is face-matching.
103
293860
1520
04:55
Now, this is a task where we're showing two images of faces
104
295420
3160
04:58
or an image and a live person,
105
298620
1640
05:00
and we're asked to decide whether they show the same person
106
300300
2760
05:03
or two different people.
107
303100
1400
05:04
Now, we've all stood in line at passport control,
108
304540
2320
05:06
anxiously awaiting the passport officer's decision
109
306900
3120
05:10
as to whether our ID photos look sufficiently like us or not.
110
310060
3640
05:14
Indeed, I've included at the top here
111
314140
2120
05:16
some examples of ID images from my own life,
112
316300
3240
05:19
just to illustrate some variability.
113
319580
2200
05:21
Some proud moments in photographic history,
114
321780
2040
05:23
I'm sure you'll agree.
115
323860
1360
05:25
And so what I'd like to do now
116
325220
1840
05:27
is first see how well you might perform as passport officers.
117
327060
3760
05:31
So here are four pairs of images,
118
331180
2160
05:33
some students’ ID images and some student photos.
119
333340
2880
05:36
For each pair, I'd like you to decide whether it's a match,
120
336260
3800
05:40
so two images of the same person,
121
340060
2040
05:42
or a mismatch,
122
342140
1160
05:43
two images of different people.
123
343340
1640
05:45
Some of you might be surprised to hear that the top two pairs are matches,
124
345420
5320
05:50
so images of the same people,
125
350740
1600
05:52
and the bottom two pairs show mismatches,
126
352380
2240
05:54
so two different people.
127
354660
1720
05:56
Now we know this task is particularly difficult
128
356700
2840
05:59
when the images show identities that we're unfamiliar with.
129
359580
3240
06:03
This is because it's hard to take into account the changes
130
363140
2720
06:05
that can happen to the face across time,
131
365900
2840
06:08
as well as over different situations,
132
368740
2040
06:10
so changes in facial expression or lighting, for instance.
133
370820
3000
06:14
We know this task is difficult for passport officers as well,
134
374620
3800
06:18
and they also make mistakes.
135
378420
2000
06:20
So this is why I thought it would be particularly interesting
136
380460
2880
06:23
to look at the relationship between insight and ability
137
383380
3000
06:26
in this important security context.
138
386380
2360
06:28
So given the issues we’ve described already
139
388740
2080
06:30
with looking at overall scores and people’s self-estimates,
140
390860
2920
06:33
I instead decided to focus on individual decision making.
141
393820
3520
06:37
So over a series of experiments,
142
397340
2120
06:39
I asked people to look at pairs of images
143
399460
2640
06:42
and decide whether they were a match or a mismatch.
144
402100
2520
06:45
But I also asked people to provide a rating of confidence in each decision.
145
405020
4280
06:49
Now a good metacognitive insight
146
409980
2240
06:52
would be reflected in people being much more confident
147
412260
3720
06:56
in decisions that turned out to be correct
148
416020
2440
06:58
and much less confident in decisions that turned out to be incorrect.
149
418500
3800
07:03
So let's have a look at how people did.
150
423300
2040
07:06
Now I think this pattern is particularly fascinating,
151
426260
2480
07:08
but also fairly intuitive.
152
428780
1720
07:10
Let's start with the red line,
153
430860
1520
07:12
which represents people's confidence in their incorrect responses.
154
432380
4280
07:17
So as you can see,
155
437020
1320
07:18
it doesn't matter how good people were at the test overall,
156
438380
3040
07:21
represented by the score on the X-axis at the bottom there;
157
441460
3320
07:24
people were approximately the same
158
444780
2360
07:27
in terms of their confidence when they were incorrect.
159
447140
3080
07:30
Now what's interesting is the blue line,
160
450580
2080
07:32
which represents confidence when people were correct in their decisions.
161
452700
4040
07:36
As you can see, the best performers on the test
162
456780
2760
07:39
were much more confident in their correct responses
163
459580
3200
07:42
in comparison with their incorrect ones.
164
462780
2360
07:45
So shows good metacognitive insight.
165
465180
2400
07:47
The weakest performers, on the other hand,
166
467860
2120
07:50
were no different in their confidence
167
470020
1880
07:51
for their correct and incorrect responses,
168
471940
2400
07:54
shown here in the green circle.
169
474340
2360
07:56
And so they show poor metacognitive insight.
170
476700
2920
07:59
So what might be going on with these weak performers?
171
479620
2720
08:02
Now it could be the case that they have some sense
172
482340
2360
08:04
they tend to perform poorly on tests in general,
173
484700
2520
08:07
and so they're just less confident overall in their responses.
174
487260
3160
08:10
However, I didn't find that pattern of lower confidence in my data,
175
490460
4160
08:14
at least with individual decision-making.
176
494620
2320
08:16
Instead, it's more likely that they were more confident in their correct responses
177
496980
4360
08:21
in comparison with their incorrect ones.
178
501380
2080
08:23
But this was simply unrelated to their accuracy on each trial
179
503820
3600
08:27
because they had poor insight.
180
507460
1920
08:29
So how does this all fit in with the Dunning-Kruger effect?
181
509820
3480
08:33
So Dunning and Kruger argued that the weakest performers
182
513820
3680
08:37
show the least amount of insight
183
517540
2320
08:39
and they overestimated their performance.
184
519860
2760
08:43
And that's implied that they had greater confidence.
185
523420
2960
08:46
Now, we didn't see that here in our data.
186
526780
3040
08:49
The weakest performers didn't seem to be overly confident.
187
529820
2880
08:53
However, the Dunning-Kruger effect
188
533020
1640
08:54
also describes how insight depends on ability.
189
534700
3760
08:59
And so the weakest performers showed the least amount of insight,
190
539020
3640
09:02
overestimating their performance in their case.
191
542660
2720
09:05
As we've just seen,
192
545780
1160
09:06
the weakest performers do seem to show the least amount of insight.
193
546940
3840
09:10
Here, they couldn't differentiate between their correct and incorrect responses.
194
550820
4160
09:15
So insight does appear to depend on ability,
195
555540
2760
09:18
but not in the way that Dunning and Kruger originally thought.
196
558340
3400
09:21
So if there are two things I'd like you to remember from this talk
197
561780
3120
09:24
and take home, think about afterwards,
198
564900
2480
09:27
they are: first, more broadly, science is always updating.
199
567380
4680
09:32
Research comes along, new evidence that may contradict
200
572100
3240
09:35
or even disprove previous work.
201
575340
2120
09:37
In this case, the Dunning-Kruger effect may well not be a thing,
202
577900
3600
09:41
despite the fact that it's so prevalent in popular culture.
203
581540
3320
09:44
Second, insight depends on ability.
204
584860
2880
09:48
For the weakest performers,
205
588100
1520
09:49
there's no difference between their confidence
206
589620
2200
09:51
for correct and incorrect responses.
207
591820
1760
09:53
They have poor insight, they can't tell the difference.
208
593580
2880
09:56
For strong performers,
209
596500
1840
09:58
when they're giving a correct answer,
210
598380
1800
10:00
they're much more confident.
211
600180
1440
10:02
Of course, the inverse isn't always true.
212
602020
2320
10:04
Being more confident doesn't mean that you're right.
213
604380
2440
10:06
You might be wrong and simply have poor insight.
214
606860
2960
10:10
So in our everyday lives,
215
610420
2040
10:12
you should think about who it is that you ask the opinions of.
216
612500
3120
10:16
If someone is an expert in their field,
217
616180
3080
10:19
then if they're more confident, they're probably right,
218
619300
3000
10:22
but if they're unsure,
219
622340
1480
10:23
this is also informative and tells us something useful.
220
623860
3240
10:27
It's much more sensible to find someone that we know is knowledgeable in an area,
221
627500
4400
10:31
rather than someone who is simply confident in their opinion,
222
631940
2880
10:34
because confidence is easily misplaced.
223
634820
2200
10:37
And finally,
224
637780
1360
10:39
for those of you who are still wondering how good my risotto actually is,
225
639180
3680
10:42
that may have to wait for a future talk.
226
642860
2280
10:45
Thank you.
227
645180
1120
10:46
(Applause)
228
646340
3400
About this website

This site will introduce you to YouTube videos that are useful for learning English. You will see English lessons taught by top-notch teachers from around the world. Double-click on the English subtitles displayed on each video page to play the video from there. The subtitles scroll in sync with the video playback. If you have any comments or requests, please contact us using this contact form.

https://forms.gle/WvT1wiN1qDtmnspy7