How Babies Think About Danger | Shari Liu | TED

43,599 views ・ 2024-02-05

TED


Please double-click on the English subtitles below to play the video.

00:03
So today I'm here to talk to you about babies
0
3801
2369
00:06
and their understanding of the world.
1
6203
2069
00:08
Specifically, their understanding of dangerous situations.
2
8272
3370
00:11
But first, in case you haven't interacted with a baby recently,
3
11675
3170
00:14
here are some clips of what we call typical one-year-old behavior.
4
14879
3803
00:18
Well, from this, you might think
5
18682
4105
00:22
that babies don't understand anything about dangerous situations.
6
22787
3270
00:26
Or maybe they don't understand anything about anything.
7
26090
2602
00:28
But today, I'm going to tell you five surprising things
8
28692
2603
00:31
that I learned while I did this work.
9
31328
2103
00:33
The first surprise is that babies are really willing to do dangerous things.
10
33964
3571
00:37
So these are videos captured in the lab.
11
37568
2936
00:40
And the finding from this work is that one-year-old babies
12
40538
3003
00:43
are perfectly willing to walk off the edge of these steep drop offs
13
43541
3703
00:47
without even thinking twice.
14
47278
1601
00:50
And in fact, they need months of experience learning to walk
15
50014
2969
00:53
before they start to show any signs of fear in these situations.
16
53017
3403
00:57
So that was what I completed before I started my PhD.
17
57288
2769
01:00
And so I looked at these findings and I thought,
18
60091
2268
01:02
Really? Is it really true that babies are oblivious to danger?
19
62393
4004
01:06
Or could we figure out another way to study this question?
20
66430
3170
01:10
So the history of developmental psychology tells us that if we study babies
21
70367
3838
01:14
by measuring complex behaviors,
22
74205
1968
01:16
we might miss out on hidden truths about their minds.
23
76207
3136
01:19
So what should we do instead?
24
79343
1435
01:20
Well, another way is, we can ask them what they think
25
80778
2769
01:23
when other people are put into the same situation.
26
83547
2503
01:26
From the first months of life,
27
86650
1469
01:28
babies have expectations about the world,
28
88152
2235
01:30
that objects are solid and don't float in mid-air,
29
90387
2470
01:32
that people have intentions and goals.
30
92890
2102
01:35
And their expectations are often clearest
31
95025
2169
01:37
when they themselves are looking at the events
32
97228
2602
01:39
rather than participating in them.
33
99864
2269
01:42
So that's going to be surprise number two.
34
102166
2069
01:44
If you test babies at the same age as I just showed you,
35
104235
2869
01:47
in the second way,
36
107104
1402
01:48
you see a completely different pattern of results.
37
108539
2636
01:52
So, here is an example of one of our stimuli.
38
112276
3403
01:56
And babies are going to see someone else, this red guy in the middle,
39
116147
3603
01:59
face choice: to jump left or jump right.
40
119750
2603
02:02
Now intuitively, you can recognize that one of these choices
41
122920
2836
02:05
is more dangerous than the other.
42
125756
1635
02:07
Falling in the ditch on the left would be a lot worse.
43
127424
2603
02:10
So all else being equal, maybe this red guy is going to go right.
44
130060
3070
02:13
And the question for babies was: can they make that distinction,
45
133130
3103
02:16
and do they have that same expectation?
46
136267
2569
02:18
So here we're measuring how long babies look at each of these outcomes.
47
138869
3637
02:22
And the simple logic of this measure is that babies,
48
142506
2469
02:24
and indeed people of all ages,
49
144975
1502
02:26
tend to pay more attention to what's surprising.
50
146510
2403
02:29
In this case, when the red guy does the more dangerous thing.
51
149313
2970
02:33
So here's what we found.
52
153184
1534
02:34
I'm plotting looking time on seconds on the vertical axis
53
154752
3069
02:37
and in red is the average looking
54
157855
2069
02:39
when babies saw the more dangerous choice, in green,
55
159957
3136
02:43
and the safer choice, in pink.
56
163127
2302
02:45
So indeed, they look longer when someone chooses a dangerous thing
57
165429
3470
02:48
over a safe thing.
58
168933
1301
02:50
But in contrast, the very same babies responded completely differently
59
170935
4337
02:55
when they saw these very, very similar control videos
60
175306
2769
02:58
with the same obstacles,
61
178108
1469
02:59
except this time there are no characters in the scene,
62
179610
2569
03:02
so there's no danger at all.
63
182179
1368
03:03
And this suggests that they're not responding to the obstacles themselves,
64
183581
3737
03:07
and they're actually really responding to the danger
65
187351
2436
03:09
that's imposed by the obstacles.
66
189787
1802
03:12
So that's the second surprise.
67
192356
1435
03:13
Babies do understand something about dangerous situations,
68
193824
2803
03:16
but remember, based on their own choices, this is shocking.
69
196660
2970
03:19
We wouldn't expect a one-year-old to care about the distinction
70
199663
2970
03:22
between a deep versus a shallow drop at all.
71
202633
2369
03:25
But measured in this way, babies are making exactly that distinction
72
205035
3437
03:28
when someone else faces the same choice.
73
208472
2336
03:32
So surprise number three is that this finding generalizes
74
212209
3003
03:35
regardless of whether babies are tested in sterile lab environments like this,
75
215246
5105
03:40
an empty room with nothing else but our videos to look at
76
220384
3103
03:43
and cluttered home environments.
77
223487
2069
03:45
So in the middle of this work, COVID hit
78
225589
1935
03:47
and developmental psychologists everywhere scrambled to figure out
79
227524
3137
03:50
whether data collection was even possible.
80
230694
2002
03:52
If you ask me, I would have predicted definitely not.
81
232730
2502
03:55
There's a reason that we conduct studies in the lab.
82
235266
2469
03:57
Our movies can be projected to these huge screens,
83
237768
2803
04:00
we can minimize distraction,
84
240571
1401
04:01
we can get highly precise,
85
241972
1669
04:03
high-definition videos of where the babies are looking.
86
243674
2869
04:07
But we figured we had to try.
87
247011
1534
04:09
So after a long process of figuring out exactly how to do this,
88
249113
3436
04:12
we ended up calling, video calling families at home,
89
252583
3170
04:15
and they played the same movies for babies on their laptop screens.
90
255753
3436
04:19
And we measured their looking behaviors just like before.
91
259189
3170
04:22
So let's just pause and take stock of all the differences between these two setups.
92
262359
4271
04:26
In many ways, this was doomed to fail.
93
266664
2035
04:28
I certainly thought it was going to fail, but that's not what happened.
94
268732
3370
04:33
So on the left are the results I just showed you,
95
273504
2302
04:35
with babies tested in the lab,
96
275839
1469
04:37
and on the right is the same experiment we repeated
97
277308
2435
04:39
in a new group of babies tested at home.
98
279777
2335
04:42
And as you can see, the main results look remarkably similar.
99
282146
3236
04:46
So that was surprise number three.
100
286984
1635
04:48
This finding is similar regardless of whether babies are tested in the lab
101
288652
3504
04:52
or at home.
102
292189
1168
04:53
So I just want to pause and state how non-trivial
103
293357
2469
04:55
and actually really groundbreaking this was for my field.
104
295859
2703
04:58
My prediction, that we cannot study babies using these measures at home,
105
298562
3871
05:02
wasn't true at all.
106
302466
1368
05:03
And during this process,
107
303867
1168
05:05
we learned not only that these studies can work at all,
108
305035
2636
05:07
but that they can actually work really well.
109
307705
2068
05:09
And at the end of the day, this process was just easier and faster for everyone.
110
309807
3870
05:14
So we ended up being able to collect video data faster than ever before,
111
314278
3537
05:17
which actually required us to engineer new tools for processing the data.
112
317848
3971
05:21
So here I'm showing you a tool we built that can find a baby's face,
113
321819
3236
05:25
given a webcam of their study
114
325089
2869
05:27
and detect where they're looking as good as a human can,
115
327991
3037
05:31
that is robust, even for these very fuzzy,
116
331061
2736
05:33
low-quality webcam videos.
117
333831
1701
05:36
So this whole experience was amazing for our team.
118
336066
3203
05:39
And for me, this experience is its own achievement.
119
339303
3036
05:42
It took families and scientists working together
120
342940
2502
05:45
to figure out how to study baby minds in a changing world.
121
345442
3103
05:49
So that was surprise number four.
122
349980
1935
05:51
The pandemic, rather than delaying and maybe ending the work in my field,
123
351949
3570
05:55
actually accelerated it.
124
355552
1502
05:57
I never would have tried online research had COVID not happened,
125
357054
3036
06:00
but now that we've tried it,
126
360124
1368
06:01
it's vastly expanded the reach of our work.
127
361525
2002
06:03
These tools enable us to study babies anywhere in the world
128
363560
2803
06:06
with an internet connection.
129
366363
1402
06:07
It lets us work with families of varying demographics,
130
367765
2602
06:10
and it lets us more easily access clinical populations
131
370367
2970
06:13
that can be harder to reach.
132
373337
1601
06:15
So the last surprise, surprise number five.
133
375272
2369
06:18
All along I've been telling you that babies look longer
134
378008
2603
06:20
when someone chooses a dangerous action
135
380644
1869
06:22
or are willing to do really dangerous things,
136
382546
2202
06:24
when I probably should have said "on average."
137
384782
2702
06:28
On average,
138
388018
1201
06:29
the babies we tested look longer at X,
139
389219
2002
06:31
on average, babies are willing to do Y.
140
391255
2602
06:34
So to illustrate this point,
141
394258
1368
06:35
here are the same graphs I showed you before,
142
395626
2135
06:37
but hidden in these graphs, and in all the data sets from my field,
143
397761
3170
06:40
are vast individual differences.
144
400964
1836
06:43
So now I've added back all the raw data that you didn't see before.
145
403200
3837
06:47
Now each pair of points is a baby.
146
407070
2236
06:49
And as you can see, there's striking variability between babies
147
409339
3003
06:52
that average out to an effect.
148
412376
1535
06:54
Now to be clear,
149
414445
1134
06:55
I don't think this undermines the conclusion of the work.
150
415612
2703
06:58
The key prediction is that at home and in the lab,
151
418315
2603
07:00
babies are sensitive to something about these dangerous situations.
152
420951
3170
07:04
But one remaining puzzle from my field
153
424455
2702
07:07
is what these individual differences mean.
154
427157
2436
07:11
So one answer is, that's just measurement noise.
155
431028
3703
07:14
We're studying babies,
156
434765
1201
07:15
they can't follow directions, they have short attention spans,
157
435999
2903
07:18
they're free to basically look as long as they want.
158
438936
2469
07:21
So maybe these differences don't mean anything.
159
441405
2202
07:23
It's just random noise.
160
443640
1302
07:24
A second answer is that part of these differences is driven by external factors.
161
444975
4037
07:29
So maybe, for our online sample,
162
449046
1902
07:30
the babies who showed the biggest effects had the biggest screens to look at,
163
450981
3637
07:34
or maybe the clearest internet connections.
164
454651
2169
07:37
And the third answer, maybe the one that's the most interesting,
165
457321
3036
07:40
is that these differences reflect something that's true
166
460390
3104
07:43
about the actual differences between these babies.
167
463527
2936
07:46
So for example, suppose that we put babies from this looking time study
168
466463
3404
07:49
in the physical situations that I showed you
169
469900
2069
07:51
from the beginning of the talk.
170
471969
1501
07:53
Remember, there are individual differences in those experiments too.
171
473504
3236
07:56
So a question could be, would the babies who show the strongest effects here
172
476773
4238
08:01
be the ones who also are more likely to be afraid
173
481044
3370
08:04
and avoid doing the dangerous things themselves?
174
484448
2269
08:06
And similarly, are the babies who do not show an effect here
175
486750
2970
08:09
the ones that are willing to kind of boldly plunge into the abyss?
176
489753
3537
08:13
So to be clear, I don't know which one of these answers is right,
177
493724
4137
08:17
but I think it's important to show this variability in our papers
178
497895
3103
08:20
and to study them.
179
500998
1468
08:22
So I think that these challenges are actually tractable,
180
502466
2869
08:25
in part because of the tools that were delivered by the pandemic.
181
505369
3069
08:28
So stay tuned and maybe I'll have an answer for you
182
508472
2402
08:30
next time we see each other.
183
510874
1468
08:33
So that's the last surprise, certainly a surprise for me,
184
513076
2703
08:35
who started out looking at bar plots of average behaviors
185
515812
2803
08:38
and assuming that most babies behave the same.
186
518649
2769
08:41
Instead, there's variability in human development
187
521852
2369
08:44
on just about any measure we can make,
188
524221
2069
08:46
on just about any topic we can study,
189
526323
2369
08:48
from physical understanding to language learning.
190
528692
2569
08:51
And making sense of these individual differences,
191
531261
2636
08:53
telling apart the signal from the noise,
192
533931
2168
08:56
continues to be a challenge and an opportunity for my field.
193
536133
2969
09:00
So the next time you look into the eyes of a baby,
194
540404
2335
09:02
know that there's way more going on in their minds and brains
195
542739
2903
09:05
than you might think.
196
545676
1167
09:06
So, zooming out to the big picture,
197
546877
1702
09:08
let's think about what these findings tell us
198
548579
2135
09:10
about the origins of the mind.
199
550747
1435
09:12
So to me, these findings, and many other findings from our field,
200
552215
3070
09:15
tell us that babies have abstract knowledge
201
555285
2002
09:17
about the social and the physical world.
202
557321
1935
09:19
They can perceive and reason about events that they've never seen before,
203
559256
3470
09:22
like these stimuli,
204
562726
1168
09:23
they can represent not only what happened
205
563927
2036
09:25
but what could happen next,
206
565963
1334
09:27
or maybe what could have happened but didn't.
207
567331
2369
09:30
And early in development, babies are learning to solve these two
208
570400
3003
09:33
really, really hard problems.
209
573437
1401
09:34
The first problem of connecting other people's actions
210
574838
3103
09:37
to their physical situations and hidden mental states.
211
577941
2936
09:40
And also the second hard problem of figuring out how to act in the world
212
580911
3403
09:44
given what they know.
213
584348
1501
09:46
So of course, that's both an interpretation and a hypothesis.
214
586216
3170
09:49
We have a lot to learn about these profoundly intelligent minds.
215
589419
3404
09:52
And actually, the magic of that process reminds me a lot of a one-year-old.
216
592856
4004
09:57
Falling over, having a long look at the obstacles before us
217
597461
3970
10:01
and then taking a big leap.
218
601465
1501
10:02
Thank you.
219
602966
1168
10:04
(Applause)
220
604167
2503
About this website

This site will introduce you to YouTube videos that are useful for learning English. You will see English lessons taught by top-notch teachers from around the world. Double-click on the English subtitles displayed on each video page to play the video from there. The subtitles scroll in sync with the video playback. If you have any comments or requests, please contact us using this contact form.

https://forms.gle/WvT1wiN1qDtmnspy7